Illuminating the Relationship Between Personality Disorder and Violence: Contributions of the General Aggression Model

Illuminating the Relationship Between Personality Disorder and Violence: Contributions of the General Aggression Model

 

Psychology of Violence © 2011 American Psychological  Association

2011, Vol. 1, No. 3,  230 –244  2152-0828/11/$12.00    DOI: 10.1037/a0024089

Flora Gilbert

This article was published Online First May 23,   2011.

Flora Gilbert, Centre for Forensic Behavioural Science, School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Monash University; Michael Daffern, Centre for Forensic Behavioural Science, School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Monash University; Victorian Institute of Forensic Mental Health; Peaks Aca- demic and Research Unit, Rampton  Hospital.

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- dressed to Flora Gilbert, Centre for Forensic Behavioural Science, 505 Hoddle Street, Clifton Hill, VIC 3068, Aus- tralia. E-mail: flora.gilbert@monash.edu

Monash University

Michael Daffern

Monash University, Victorian Institute of Forensic Mental Health, and Rampton  Hospital

Although a consensus exists among mental health professionals that certain personality disorders are associated with an increased propensity for violence, uncertainties re- garding the nature and extent of this relationship persist. Previous approaches to studying the personality disorder-violence relationship are also hampered by an array of methodological issues. This paper presents a novel approach to enhanced under- standing of the personality disorder-violence relationship, by integrating contemporary psychological aggression theory with the extant personality disorder-violence literature. A theoretical conceptualization based on the most comprehensive and contemporary psychological theory of aggression, the General Aggression Model (GAM), is pro- posed. The GAM was developed to explain individual variations in aggression pro- pensity, and specifies several key constructs that are important in influencing aggres- sion: aggression-related knowledge structures, maladaptive cognitions, and anger. Results of this analysis support the notion that people with those personality disorders that are associated with violence have more of these constructs, though the empirical evidence is strongest for Antisocial Personality Disorder, and a number of constructs, including violent scripts, have rarely been studied. The conceptualization offered here provides a focus for researchers to further elucidate the relationship between person- ality disorder and violence and for clinicians to more systematically assess relevant constructs to determine violence potential in people with personality disorder, and also, to focus their violence reduction efforts on relevant psychological constructs. This analysis also calls into question the use of personality disorder per se for risk assess- ment or as a criterion by which to determine inclusion or exclusion from mental health treatment or incapacitation.

Mental health professionals and criminal jus- tice personnel have long considered individuals with certain personality disorders to pose an increased risk of violence. Over the last two decades a large body of literature has accumu- lated to support this contention, with findings consistently revealing higher rates of aggressive and violent behavior in male and female offend- ers with personality disorder (Blackburn & Coid, 1999; Hodgins & Cote, 1993; Logan & Blackburn, 2009; Warren, Burnette, et al., 2002; Warren, Hurt, et al., 2002). However, signifi- cant problems in understanding the relationship between personality disorder and violence per- sist, resulting in substantial shortcomings in the extant  literature.  This  paper  presents  a novel approach  to  elucidating  the  personality disorder-violence relationship. It contends that a con- temporary psychological theory of aggression, the General Aggression Model (GAM; Ander- son & Bushman, 2002; Anderson & Carnagey, 2004; Anderson, Gentile, & Buckley, 2007) that was developed to explain variations in aggres- sion propensity, ought to be drawn on to (1) elaborate on the link between personality disor- der and violence, (2) provide a systematic   way for clinicians to investigate relevant aggression- related phenomena, and (3) to focus interven- tions to reduce violence  potential.

The paper begins with a brief outline of rel- evant literature, before noting the ways in which research on the relationship between personality disorder and aggression is compromised. It then describes the GAM, focusing on the key cogni- tive and affective factors that are proposed to influence aggression propensity. Finally, it ex- amines whether evidence is available to confirm an increased incidence of key constructs con- tained within this model, in particular aggres- sion-related cognitions, in people with person- ality disorder, and the implications of these findings to the assessment and treatment of vi- olent individuals with personality disorder, or who are considered to be at risk for   violence.

 

Evidence for a Relationship Between Personality Disorder and Violence

Prisoners

A diagnosis of Antisocial Personality Disor- der (ASPD) has consistently been linked to the criminal behavior, including violent offending, of prisoners (e.g., Hodgins & Cote, 1993; Rob- erts & Coid, 2009); it also predicts violent and disruptive incidents while in prison (Friedmann, Melnick, Jiang, & Hamilton, 2008; O’Brien, Mortimer, Singleton, & Meltzer, 2003). Two studies examining rates of personality disorder among offenders convicted of serious violent offending found that the vast majority met cri- teria for at least one personality disorder, where the rates of ASPD, Borderline (BPD), Narcis- sistic (NPD), and Paranoid (PPD) Personality Disorders ranged from 47 to 69% (Blackburn & Coid, 1999; Coid, 1998). Similarly, in a sample of male prisoners transferred to special prison units in England as a result of disruptive behav- ior, Coid (2002) found rates of between 56 and 84% for the same four  disorders.

Psychiatric Patients

Patients detained in forensic psychiatric hos- pitals are also more likely to meet diagnostic criteria for personality disorder when compared with the general population and these patients are also more likely to reoffend violently after discharge. For example, Tardiff, Marzuk, Leon, and Portera (1997) found that patients with BPD or ASPD were four times more likely, and pa- tients with any other personality disorder were more than two times more likely, to be violent after discharge from a psychiatric hospital than those without personality disorder. Higher rates of violent behavior have also been documented in patients with personality disorder admitted to psychiatric wards (Hodgins, Mednick, Brennan, Schulsinger, & Engberg, 1996), in contact with public psychiatric services (Wallace et al., 1998) and entering substance abuse treatment (Hernandez-Avila et al., 2000).

 

Other Violent Individuals

Further evidence for a relationship between personality disorder and violence has emerged from research investigating perpetrators of vio- lence in the community. A large number of cross-sectional studies have demonstrated that individuals who engage in violent and nonvio- lent offending (Barros & Serafim, 2008; Thorn- ton, Graham-Kevan, & Archer, 2010), aggres- sion (Berman, Fallon, & Coccaro, 1998; Yang & Coid, 2007), and intimate partner violence (Ross & Babcock, 2009; South, Turkheimer, & Oltmanns, 2008), are more likely to meet diag- nosis for a personality disorder. These patterns have also been reflected in longitudinal data. For instance, Johnson et al. (2000) found that individuals diagnosed with a cluster A or B personality disorder, as defined by the Diagnos- tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM–IV–TR; American Psychi- atric Association, 2000), had a threefold likeli- hood of committing violent acts in the future. A high propensity for aggression has also been identified in individuals seeking treatment for personality disorder (Critchfield, Levy, Clarkin, & Kernberg, 2008; Howard, Huband, Duggan, & Mannion, 2008).

 

Violent Recidivism

 Personality disorder is a central criterion in current approaches to violence risk assessment and management, and has been documented to increase the risk of violent recidivism (Bonta, Law, & Hanson, 1998; Hiscoke, Langström, Ottosson, & Grann, 2003). A diagnosis of ASPD is especially predictive, for instance, His- coke et al. (2003) found that reconviction  rates for attempted or completed murder, manslaugh- ter, assault, robbery, or rape were 3.7 times higher for individuals with this diagnosis. Psy- chopathy has also been found to strongly predict violent recidivism (Grann, Langstrom, Teng- strom, & Kullgren, 1999).

 

Barriers to Precise Characterization of the Personality Disorder and Violence Relationship

Limitations of Personality Disorder Constructs and Measurement

The abovementioned studies incorporate a range of different populations and methodolo- gies and collectively support the existence of a relationship between certain personality disor- ders and violence, with the strongest evidence appearing to relate to four personality disorders: ASPD, BPD, NPD, and PPD. However, there are inherent difficulties associated with explor- ing patterns of violence in people with person- ality disorder, limiting the assumptions about the violent behavior of this  group.

Overlapping and equally weighted criteria in the current diagnostic system. A major limitation of research in this field stems from the inherent and widely acknowledged difficul- ties that are present in our existing classification of personality disorder. In the prevailing diag- nostic system, the DSM–IV–TR (American Psy- chiatric Association, 2000), angry and aggres- sive behavior is specified as a defining feature in two of the 10 disorders (ASPD and BPD), with antagonistic and hostile traits noted in seven categories. The present nomenclature has often been criticized for the lack of a cohesive, pro- totypic hierarchy of characteristics and for giv- ing equal weight to criteria that may be less central to the personality disorder they define (McGlashan et al., 2005). In particular, given the polythetic nature of the criteria, it is impos- sible to determine whether an actual disposition toward aggressive or violent acts is inferred by any diagnosis. It is also unclear whether aggres- sive behavior is the product of overall person- ality pathology, or linked to particular person- ality characteristics that manifest heteroge- neously across the disorders. Because the DSM– IV–TR is currently undergoing significant change and is proposing a move toward rating dimensions of personality traits (American Psychiatric Association, 2011), reexamination of the role of personality dysfunction and specific psychological constructs associated with ag- gression is especially timely.

Comorbidity of diagnoses. Another tenu- ous assumption is that the categories of person- ality disorder traits are etiologically distinct rather than co-occurring. In both forensic and nonforensic populations, research suggests that the most common pattern observed is one of high comorbidity, both within and across the three DSM–IV–TR clusters (Zimmerman, 2005). Consequently, the “individual” relation- ship between each disorder and aggression is confounded.

Methodological difficulties.  There  are also a range of methodological problems asso- ciated with research in this area. Principally, the assessment of personality disorder itself contin- ues to be contentious. For instance, there tends to be a lack of agreement between different assessment instruments (Tyrer et al., 2007), and poor correspondence between self-report and interview methods of assessment (Clark, Lives- ley, & Morey, 1997). With regard to the empir- ical exploration of the relationship between personality disorder and violence, studies are frequently limited by focusing only on the dis- orders widely assumed to be linked to violence (Thornton et al., 2010), the use of small or unrepresentative samples, the use of violence in the establishment of the personality disorder diagnosis and as a dependent variable, and the reliance on cross-sectional assessment methods, which fail to capture the stability of personality disorder and violence over time. There is also a relative lack of research using female  samples.

 

Limitations in the Measurement of Aggression and Violence

 Considerable progress has been made in psy- chological knowledge regarding aggression and violence, and there is increasing acknowledg- ment that violent behavior results from a com- plex interaction of individual and contextual factors, the relative importance of which varies across person, time, and situation (Widiger & Trull, 1994). In the past decade, research has confirmed the existence of various types of ag- gression that are accompanied by differing un- derlying motivations (McEllistrem, 2004; Weinshenker  &  Siegel,  2002).  Recent studies have elaborated on the role of impulsivity in relation to both hostile and instrumental forms of aggression (Howard, 2009), and investigated the differing manifestations of violence in males and females (Thornton et al., 2010). The potential confounding effect of different kinds of aggression may account for the conflicting or inconclusive results that are frequently ob- served in the personality disorder-violence lit- erature. Finally, a recent study demonstrated that the aggression of individuals with person- ality disorder serves a range of different func- tions that differ across environments (Daffern & Howells, 2009), and further emphasizes the im- portance of more sophisticated levels of under- standings of violence to this category of disor- ders.

 

Social Psychological Theories of Aggression and Violence

 Although a number of theoretical accounts of the specific processes contributing to violent behavior in personality disorder have emerged (e.g., Duggan & Howard, 2009; Howard, 2009), development of a common theoretical frame- work is required to guide the investigation of the personality disorder-violence  relationship. A logical approach would be to commence with an overarching conceptualization of aggressive behavior. In this sense, application of the well- established social– cognitive literature regard- ing personality and aggression may offer valu- able insight into the distinct psychological features that characterize those with personality disorder and a propensity for  violence.

Social cognition is based on the principle that constructs relevant to cognitive represen- tation and process are fundamental to under- standing all human responses, regardless of whether those responses are social or nonso- cial in nature (Ostrom, 1994). Our cognitions shape our perceptions of the environment around us and the behavioral responses that are deemed to be appropriate (Huesmann, 1988). Contemporary theoretical and investi- gative approaches to aggression are guided by the notion that individual differences in ag- gressive tendencies are, to a large extent, de- termined by the influence of an underlying set of cognitive variables. These variables are understood to include cognitive-processing, negative  affect,  self-regulation,  and   social-

information processing (Bettencourt, Talley, Benjamin, & Valentine, 2006). Prominent and informative theories of aggression suggest that individuals with a propensity toward ag- gressive behavior hold more elaborate and readily accessible aggression-related cogni- tions (Anderson & Bushman, 2002; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Huesmann,  1998).

 

The GAM

The most contemporary and comprehensive theory of aggression, the GAM (Anderson & Bushman, 2002; Anderson & Carnagey, 2004; Anderson et al., 2007) is notable in that it draws on existing theoretical approaches such as cognitive neoassociation (Berkowitz, 1990) and social learning (Bandura, 1977), and unifies them. The GAM contends that aggressive acts rarely occur without the con- vergence of multiple precipitating situational factors and predisposing personal character- istics, and identifies the various constructs  that operate in the current situation to initiate aggression alongside those that exert an influence over a longer period of time. Ag- gressive behavior is therefore understood as resulting from a series of experiences that prepare the individual to behave aggressively in different situations, culminating in the ac- quisition of aggressive cognitive structures that are then repeatedly retrieved and   used.

While an intention to harm is the immediate, proximate, goal of aggression, the ultimate goals may differ (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). For example, acts may primarily have an instru- mental or affective basis, or may contain a combination of motives. Emerging research tends to be consistent with this overall view, supporting the notion that characteristically ag- gressive individuals have more extensive and developed cognitive networks linked to aggres- sion (see Collie, Vess, & Murdoch, 2007, for a review). Figure 1 describes the main focus of the GAM, representing the (1) person and situ- ation factors influencing aggression  likelihood,

  • the internal routes through which input vari- ables influences the present internal state,  and
  • the underlying appraisal and decision- making process that determines behavioral outcomes.

 

Figure 1. The general aggression model episodic pro- cesses (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). Copyright 2002 by Annual Reviews.

Personality as Schema

While the GAM posits important roles for in- nate biological tendencies and situational factors, it characterizes personality as the sum of the knowledge structures an individual constructs from their experiences (Anderson & Carnagey, 2004). These structures, more widely referred to as schema, exist on a macro level to represent substantial knowledge about a concept, its attri- butes, and relationship to other concepts (Hues- mann, 1998). The form and content of knowledge structures influence how the environment is per- ceived, and are used to guide people’s interpreta- tions and behavioral responses to their social en- vironment. Knowledge structures also influence which types of situations are sought out and which are avoided, contributing to trait-like consistency in behavior.

 

Aggression-Related Knowledge Structures

Huesmann’s (1998) formulation of aggres- sive schema is incorporated into the GAM and describes in detail the specific types of knowl- edge structure that shape individual aggression propensity. Huesmann proposes that there are two important kinds of aggression-related sche- matic content. The first is aggressive behavioral scripts, which are acquired through observa- tions of others, and serve to define situations and guide behavior. The process of use and positive reinforcement of aggressive scripts causes them to become firmly established as structures, and as a consequence, they become well-rehearsed  and  highly  accessible. Habitu-

ally aggressive people are those individuals who regularly retrieve and use aggressive scripts for social behavior.

The second kind of knowledge structure in- fluencing the likelihood of aggression is norma- tive beliefs, which consist of cognitions about the perceived appropriateness of the behavior (Huesmann, 1998). These beliefs serve to guide the evaluation of social behavior and the search for appropriate behavioral scripts, effectively operating as a filter that regulates aggression. Although the nature of the beliefs is related to perceived social norms, they ultimately reflect what is suitable for the individual. The proper- ties of scripts and normative beliefs, and their mode of operation, were recently described elsewhere (Gilbert & Daffern,  2010).

 

The Role of Affective Arousal in Aggression

A further determinant of behavioral action is the affect-based rules that are linked to an indi- vidual’s aggression-related knowledge struc- tures. Anger, as well as other types of affective arousal (e.g., fear, anxiety, depression), can serve as cues for retrieval of related schemas and scripts. In addition, according to the GAM, unrelated states of arousal may increase domi- nant behavioral tendencies such as aggression; and high or low levels of affective arousal may be aversive states that stimulate aggressive re- sponses (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). Hues- mann (1998) further contends that anger re- stricts cognitive processing so that only the most accessible scripts are retrieved, meaning that if an aggressive repertoire is entrenched, it will be enacted.

 

Evidence for the GAM Constructs in Those Personality Disorders Associated With Violence

The GAM was developed with the intention of explaining enduring differences in aggres- siveness, and thus may help to clarify the relationship between personality disorder and violence. This section integrates the existing personality disorder literature relating to four areas of significance from a GAM perspective: maladaptive cognitions, aggressive behavioral scripts,  aggression-supportive  beliefs,  and anger. The purpose of this review is to answer two questions, (1) because the GAM provides a broad understanding of aggression, can this framework account for the increased aggres- siveness of those individuals with the personal- ity disorders that have an established link with violence; and (2) does the application of the GAM to personality disorder elucidate the rela- tionship between these disorders and  violence?

 

Cognitive Schema and Personality  Disorder

The characteristic emotional dysfunction and maladaptive behavior associated with personal- ity disorder has been conceptualized as being maintained by maladaptive cognitive schema (Beck, Freeman, & Associates, 1990). Particu- larly influential has been the work of Young and colleagues (Young, 1994, 1999; Young, Klo- sko, & Weishaar, 2003), which proposes that personality pathology is initiated by rigid core beliefs called Early Maladaptive Schema (EMS). EMS develop early in life as relatively realistic representations of, and effective adap- tations to, the environment, becoming elabo- rated on throughout the lifetime. Young et al. (2003) contend that the dysfunctional behaviors represented by personality disorder develop in response to the presence of  EMS.

In line with the GAM, a range of EMS may well be implicated in aggression. Common themes of hostility and emotional pain and dis- comfort underpin a large number of EMS, aspects which the GAM suggests are key activators of aggression-related knowledge structures (Ander- son & Bushman, 2002). In addition, the presence of EMS can be speculated to contribute to chronic aggressive behavior. Young et al. (2003) propose that the triggering of EMS, through situational factors such as interpersonal provocation or failure to attain a goal, becomes a threat representing a frustration of a core emotional need (e.g., secure attachment) and the prospect of experiencing in- tensely painful emotions. However, because of the need for cognitive consistency, incoming informa- tion is distorted to maintain the validity of the EMS. The tendency to experience this combina- tion of internal discomfort and distorted informa- tion-processing may increase aggression propen- sity over the long-term, whereby aggression emerges as a coping style aimed at restoring a more tolerable internal state.

Relatively few studies have examined the empirical relationship between schema content and personality disorder in clinical samples.  The available literature does, however, reveal that BPD has been linked to a diverse range of EMS, with modest schema configurations across the other personality disorders (Ball & Cecero, 2001; Jovev & Jackson, 2004; Nilsson, Jorgensen, Straarup, & Licht, 2010; Nordahl, Holthe, & Haugum, 2005). Moreover, two stud- ies suggest that investigation of the role of EMS in prompting or maintaining violent behavior in people with personality disorder is warranted. Specht, Chapman, and Cellucci (2009) recently examined the relationship between schema and BPD symptoms in a sample of female prisoners. Although the nature of the offenses committed by the sample were not described, the authors found that two schema domains, ‘Disconnection and Rejection’ and ‘Impaired Limits,’ were sig- nificantly correlated with BPD severity. An- other study investigated the association between EMS and trait aggressiveness. In a nonclinical sample, Tremblay and Dozois (2009) found sig- nificant relationships for the majority of EMS, although those most strongly associated were ‘Mistrust and Abuse,’ ‘Insufficient Self- Control,’ and ‘Entitlement.’ Schema configura- tions have yet to be examined in violent offend- ers with personality disorder, meaning that relatively little is understood regarding the schematic content and processes involved in their cognitive functioning.

 

Aggressive Scripts

Investigation of aggressive scripts is cur- rently limited by the lack of standardized mea- sures available for this purpose. As noted by Collie et al. (2007), the presence and form of cognitive structures must be inferred from self- report and cognitive paradigms. A range of dif- ferent methodologies have been utilized to date, including examination of offense narratives (Polaschek & Gannon, 2004), self-report mea- sures of aggressive scripts (Grisso, Davis, Ves- selinov, Appelbaum, & Monahan, 2000), and implicit association tasks (Gray, MacCulloch, Smith, Morris, & Snowden, 2003). Although several studies have specifically examined the importance of aggressive behavioral scripts in aggressive children (e.g., Guerra, Huesmann, & Spindler, 2003), there is currently only one standardized measure in existence that is appropri- ate for use with adults. This measure, the Sched- ule of Imagined Violence (SIV), was developed by Grisso et al. (2000) to examine the presence of violent mental imagery in patients hospital- ized with mental disorder. The SIV assesses whether or not a person has thoughts about physically hurting other people, and the quality of these thoughts (e.g., frequency, chronicity). Using the SIV, Grisso et al. found that the degree of violent thought rehearsal reported by patients was predictive of violent acts occurring after discharge. The SIV has also been used to explore the relationship between aggressive scripts and aggression in a nonclinical sample, where a significant positive association was also observed (Nagtegaal, Rassin, & Muris, 2006). These findings support the GAMs proposal that social situations are more likely to trigger ag- gressive cognitive content among people for whom the rehearsal of aggressive scripts is more frequent.

The findings of Coid (2002) are also note- worthy. Coid examined the relationship be- tween personality pathology and motivations for severe disruptive behavior in a sample of prisoners in the U.K. He found that individuals who met criteria for psychopathy, NPD, PPD, or Histrionic Personality Disorder (HPD) dis- played a range of violent behaviors that were motivated by pride in physical prowess and fighting skills. According to the GAM, these motivations would be associated with activation of aggressive knowledge structures, particularly the rehearsal of aggressive scripts. Frequent re- hearsal of aggressive scripts renders the script more accessible, increasing the likelihood of enactment of violent acts.

 

Normative Beliefs Supportive of  Aggression

Research investigating the relationship be- tween antisocial attitudes and offending is well established, and there has been a proliferation in the methods available for this purpose. How- ever, few studies have examined the normative beliefs about aggression held by those with per- sonality disorder. Investigation of the motiva- tional factors for offending in this group provides some insight into the significance of beliefs about the appropriateness of violence. For example, in Coid’s (2002) study, the vari- ous disruptive behaviors exhibited by  prisoners

with psychopathy, and NPD, PPD, HPD, and Passive-Aggressive Personality Disorder, were in part motivated by a belief that violence was the only solution to interpersonal problems. In contrast, the belief was not associated with the disruptive behavior of prisoners with BPD. This study seems to suggest that normative beliefs supportive of violence may partly precipitate the violence of those with personality disorder. With respect to the individual personality dis- orders, it would be expected that individuals with ASPD, who are described as displaying a pervasive pattern of disregard for the rights of others (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), would be especially likely to hold atti- tudes that support the use of aggression. Ag- gression-supportive beliefs are broadly predic- tive of violent offending and violent recidivism (Archer & Haigh, 1997; Mills, Kroner, & Hem- mati, 2004). A study by Polaschek, Calvert, and Gannon (2009) examined in detail the various beliefs associated with violent offending by an- alyzing the transcripts of offense process inter- views for men convicted of violent offenses. A commonly held belief was the “normalization” of violence, in which it was seen as a routine occurrence that was helpful in achieving per- sonal and social goals. This assumption also underpinned more specific violence-supportive beliefs, such as the need to act violently to achieve or maintain status in a violent world, and a moral entitlement to harm other people if required. Given the high rates of ASPD in vio- lent offenders, it is likely that these beliefs are also endorsed in those violent offenders meet- ing criteria for ASPD. In further support of a relationship between normative beliefs support- ive of violence and ASPD, Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart’s (1994) research into males perpe- trating domestic violence describes two sub- types characterized, respectively, by borderline and antisocial personality disorder traits. The researchers found that although these subtypes displayed separate profiles of risk factors for violence and differed in the nature and extent of their violent behavior, they could both be dis- tinguished from other domestically violent men by the presence of positive attitudes to violence (Holtzworth-Munroe,   Meehan,   Herron, Rehman, & Stuart, 2000).

Additional evidence is available from the psychopathy literature. Psychopathy and ASPD share  characteristics,  although  the  latter  is  a more narrowly defined personality disorder in which behavioral traits predominate (Ogloff, 2006). The literature supports a strong relation- ship between psychopathy and diverse forms of violence, including those acts underpinned by instrumental goals (Cornell et al., 1996). Blair (1995) notes that cognitive-affective processes that would normally inhibit violence, such as moral remorse and empathy, are distinctively lacking in psychopathy. In support of this, studies have confirmed a relationship between psychopathy and various criminal cognitions (Seager, 2005; Simourd & Hoge, 2000). For example, using a form of implicit association to examine the strength of associations between violent concepts in memory in psychopathic murderers, Gray et al. (2003) found that violent words were closely associated with positive evaluations. This suggests that these individuals hold positive cognitions about  violence.

Research into beliefs about violence held by those with other personality disorders is unfor- tunately lacking. Aggression-supportive beliefs may, however, be tentatively linked to two other personality disorder categories. The GAM iden- tifies narcissism as a proximate cause of aggres- sion (Anderson & Carnagey, 2004). Studies in nonclinical populations have confirmed the as- sociation between aggression-supportive beliefs and narcissistic traits, with these beliefs predict- ing delinquent behavior in adolescents (Ang, Ong, Lim, & Lim, 2010; Calvete, 2008). In addition, Calvete and Orue (2010) found that justification of violence and grandiosity sche- mas were more relevant for proactive, instru- mental aggression than angry, reactive acts. Other, more specific, schema that are theoreti- cally associated with the acceptance of violence have also been found to predict aggression, for example, rigid gender role beliefs (Parrott, Pe- terson, & Bakeman, 2011), although these have yet to be examined in relation to the construct of personality disorder.

The suspicious and hostile expectations of abuse and mistreatment described in PPD may also result in a tendency toward beliefs support- ing violence. Howells (2009) notes that the cog- nitive components of this disorder are compa- rable to the cognitive and attributional biases that characterize angry aggressive reactions, such as the attribution of malevolent intent, rehearsal of grudges, blaming, and a lack of forgiveness. This relationship has not been   in-

vestigated empirically, although one study has investigated the presence of an aggressive attri- butional style, comprising hostile attributions, perceptions of threat and the appropriateness of violence, in psychiatric inpatients (McNeil, Eis- ner, & Binder, 2003). It found that a cognitive style comprising these features was associated with increased levels of violence, suggesting that perceptions of hostility and threat may op- erate to justify violent behavior in a particular context, increasing the likelihood of its occur- rence. Equivalent research is required in people meeting criteria for PPD.

 

Anger

 Under the GAM, the presence of highly arousing interpersonal situations are understood to interfere with higher-level cognitive pro- cesses and reduce inhibitions by providing a justification for aggressive retaliation. Anger is an important affective antecedent for aggres- sion, and four of the 10 DSM–IV–TR personal- ity disorders make explicit reference to patho- logical anger: ASPD, BPD, NPD, and PPD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Re- cent research findings are consistent with the notion that anger is associated with the aggres- sion of individuals with personality disorder. For instance, Posternak and Zimmerman (2002) examined experiences of subjective anger and aggression in a large sample of psychiatric out- patients. They found that individuals with a cluster B personality disorder were almost five times more likely to report subjective anger compared to those without this diagnosis. In a study of the functions of aggression in inpa- tients at a high secure Dangerous and Severe Personality Disorder service in the U.K., Daf- fern and Howells (2009) also found that anger expression was the most common function of aggression across patients’ violent index of- fenses and acts of inpatient  aggression.

Independently, it appears that BPD demon- strates the strongest association with anger. A robust link is consistently observed in clinical studies (e.g., Costa & Babcock, 2008), although there is evidence to suggest that anger is more prevalent in males with a diagnosis of BPD than females (Tadic et al., 2009). The literature also indicates that the violence perpetrated by indi- viduals with BPD may be broadly underpinned by the emotion regulation difficulties that characterize the disorder (Critchfield et al., 2008; Warren & South, 2009).

In relation to ASPD, study findings relating to anger are inconsistent. Research rarely ex- cludes those who also meet diagnostic criteria for psychopathy (de Brito & Hodgins, 2009), which is problematic since a deficient affective experience, rather than pathological anger, is a major dimension in contemporary measures of psychopathy. Consequently, the extent to which violence perpetrated by people with ASPD is driven by anger and difficulty controlling anger remains unclear. In a study by DiGiuseppe and Tafrate (2004), ASPD produced only a modest correlation with anger in a clinical sample, lower than that for other personality disorders. In contrast, other studies have documented a strong relationship between ASPD and anger (e.g., Wang & Diamond, 1999). These differing outcomes may indicate that anger is not a crit- ical instigator for violence in people with ASPD, because there are other traits that are present acting as cognitive mechanisms to fa- cilitate violent acts. For example, recent evi- dence suggests that anger experienced by those with ASPD may reflect a strong, controlled, predatory component (Lobbestael, Arntz, Cima, & Chakhssi, 2009). This is consistent with the recent hypothesis by Howard (2009) that offen- sive violence, accompanied by pleasant affect, may be more common in those with ASPD. Nevertheless, the research overall seems to sug- gest heterogeneity across individuals with ASPD with respect to the relationship between their affective experience and violent behavior. A recent contention has been that the combi- nation of ASPD and BPD has a particularly strong relationship with violent offending (Sheldon & Krishnan, 2009). These disorders share anger as a central criterion, and recent research suggests that this may be linked with an increased propensity for violence. Howard et al. (2008) examined the personality and crimi- nal history of individuals in the community with a combination of these personality disorders. They found that high levels of emotional dys- regulation, impulsivity and anger expression, and low anger control were associated with a history of violence. A recent study also found that when controlling for criminal history and severity of violence, men with BPD were more likely to engage in angry, reactive partner vio- lence, whereas men with ASPD used violence both proactively and reactively (Ross & Bab- cock, 2009), suggesting that the affective pre- cipitants of these disorders cannot be mapped directly onto each other.

Finally, the GAM contends that a major fea- ture of NPD, inflated self-esteem, is associated with anger and aggression if this self-image is threatened (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). Al- though there is a comparative lack of research in relation to NPD, the existing literature appears to indicate that the anger of these individuals has a narrow relationship with triggering or provoking events. In nonclinical populations, individuals with narcissistic traits have been found to respond with anger in response to feedback informing them of their failure on tasks (Rhodewalt & Morf, 1998), while a series of studies by Twenge and Campbell (2003) found that individuals high in narcissism were more likely to respond to an experience of so- cial rejection with anger. Because the GAM suggests that anger enables a person to sustain aggressive intentions over the long-term and provides a justification for aggressive retalia- tion, further research exploring the conditions in which anger and aggression are deemed to be appropriate appears necessary. Replication of existing findings in clinical samples and in in- dividuals with established histories of respond- ing aggressively would be particularly  useful.

 

Summary

The assumption that certain personality dis- orders are associated with an increased propen- sity for violence has been repeatedly estab- lished, although several methodological issues, including measurement, definitional and con- struct related problems, continue to obscure and overshadow this relationship. The current anal- ysis has consequently drawn on the GAM, a comprehensive theoretical model that provides an account of the development of aggressive- ness and the operation of cognitive variables important to aggression, to elucidate the rela- tionship between personality disorder and vio- lence. Application of the GAM to the personal- ity disorder-aggression relationship assists with the organization of the vast array of existing research, and will hopefully generate further research interest in this area. The current anal- ysis highlights the deficit in research into par- ticular constructs. For this conceptualization  to be wholly validated there will be a need to more fully examine the existence and operation of the GAM-related constructs in a range of popula- tions with personality disorder.

The GAMs main contention is that people who hold entrenched and accessible aggression- related cognitions, and who are susceptible to experiencing the internal states that activate these cognitions, are more prone to act aggres- sively. With respect to aggression-related knowledge structures, an increasing amount of evidence is available to confirm a relationship between normative beliefs supportive of aggres- sion and ASPD. For two personality disorders, NPD and PPD, the presence of a relationship with these beliefs remains speculative, while for BPD, a relationship is entirely uncertain. The research more broadly suggests, in line with the GAM, that individuals who believe that aggres- sive behavior will produce the desired outcome, and who believe that they can successfully carry out aggressive acts, are more likely to be ag- gressive (Anderson & Bushman,  2002).

The second type of knowledge structure, ag- gressive scripts, is the area that is least re- searched. Whether people with those personal- ity disorders linked to violence are more likely to hold and rehearse aggressive behavioral scripts has yet to be investigated. As such, it is unclear to what extent the relationship between personality disorder and violence is accounted for by the presence of aggressive  scripts.

In terms of maladaptive cognitions, EMS re- search substantiates their importance to person- ality disorder more generally. BPD has received the most empirical attention and has been linked to a diverse range of EMS. Research that has examined the significance of schema in individ- uals with other personality disorders displaying a proneness to violence is rare, however, mak- ing conclusions about the underlying anteced- ents for violence in this group difficult. The GAM theorizes that maladaptive cognitions are an important component influencing aggression propensity, and Young’s conceptualization of EMS appears to lend itself well to further ex- ploration of the relationship.

The GAM further contends that affective arousal is an important instigator of aggression. Examination of the literature confirms that an- ger is frequently associated with the violent behavior of people with various personality dis- orders. However, it also tends to challenge the assumption that anger invariably precipitates this violence. Violent acts may, instead, be ini- tiated by other personality characteristics that reinforce the use of well-rehearsed and accessi- ble aggression-related cognitions (e.g., a re- stricted affective experience). Both of these kinds of violence have been observed in stud- ies of people with ASPD, suggesting that the diagnostic criteria for this disorder alone have limited explanatory value with respect to the significance of anger and affective arousal to patterns of violence. Nonetheless, elevated levels of anger, does, from a GAM perspec- tive, account in part for the increased prone- ness to  violence of  this group.

Overall, the accumulating evidence seems to support the notion that people with personality disorders who are violent demonstrate more of the key cognitive and affective characteristics central to aggression as conceptualized by the GAM, that is, aggression-related cognitions, maladaptive cognitions, and anger. It appears that of all the personality disorders, the relation- ship between ASPD and violence is best ac- counted for by the increased presence of the various constructs of the GAM. The evidence for the other personality disorders linked to violence is less clear, primarily because these important aggression-related constructs have not been adequately studied. The GAM pro- vides a logical theoretical counterpart for clini- cians tasked with assessing violence propensity in people with personality disorder. More thor- ough assessment of the cognitive and affective factors associated with aggression is likely to assist in differentiating individuals coming un- der the broad category of personality disorder, in that this approach helps to overcome the difficulties associated with focusing on the di- agnostic categories themselves.

 

Research Implications

To increase the clinical applicability of the GAM to the assessment and treatment of indi- viduals with personality disorder, there are a number of important research implications. Several of these concern the investigation of individual aspects of the GAM, which would assist with development of a more robust re- search foundation upon which to base clinical applications. Most importantly, further research into aggressive scripts is required. Given that aggressive scripts have a critical role in the GAM, the lack of research in this area inhibits overall progress in theoretical understandings of the relationship between personality disorder and violence. As such, it will be necessary to investigate the role of aggressive scripts in in- dividuals with personality disorder with estab- lished histories of violent behavior. This would enable a test of the overall assumption that individuals with personality disorder who are more violent hold more significant and en- trenched aggression-related cognitions, helping to account for the variations in violence propen- sity observed in people with these  diagnoses.

Research should also further examine the normative beliefs about aggression and violence held by individuals with personality disorder. It seems unlikely that the operation of these be- liefs would be the same across the different personality disorders; for instance, there may be differences in the types of situations in which violence is perceived to be acceptable. Research in samples of people with BPD, NPD, and PPD is especially needed. In addition, because vari- ous studies have demonstrated that maladaptive schema can be quantified with relative ease, further investigation in personality disorder populations displaying increased levels of vio- lence is required. This seems particularly im- portant given the increasing application of sche- ma-focused therapy to offending populations. Finally, future research will need to explore the role of anger in relation to ASPD while control- ling for psychopathy, and examine the presence of other types of affective arousal with respect to violence in BPD.

The other main research implication is that it will be important for studies to investigate how maladaptive cognitions, aggression-related knowledge structures and anger and other affec- tive states interact with each other and are linked to violent behavior in people with differ- ent personality disorders. The GAM contends that the existence of one of these constructs would increase the likelihood of activation and retrieval of the other constructs, because of the strengthened associations between aggression- related cognitions. The relative importance or combined effect of the constructs may differ in individuals with personality disorder compared to those without these diagnoses, and studies should specifically examine violent offender populations in which high rates of    personality disorder are common. Given that the GAM re- mains largely theoretical in nature, this in turn may assist with refinement of the model as it currently stands.

 

Clinical and Policy Implications

The policy implications of this analysis are that clinical assessment of the central tenets of the GAM, that is, aggression-related cognitions, maladaptive schema and anger, in offenders with personality disorder would be highly use- ful in explaining the increased violence propen- sity of individuals with these diagnoses. For those personality disorders with a documented relationship with violence, such as ASPD and BPD, the importance of individual case formu- lation of maladaptive cognitions and other ag- gression-related knowledge structures may be helpful. More standardized assessment of these various constructs would undoubtedly be useful for treatment planning. This would assist the demarcation of factors that are closely tied to violence propensity and thus, should be a spe- cific focus of treatment. In particular, interven- tions that change aggression-supportive cogni- tions (e.g., the Violence Reduction Program; Wong, 2000), schema-focused therapy (Bern- stein, Arntz, & de Vos, 2007), and anger man- agement (e.g., Watt & Howells, 1999) might all be needed, depending on the significance of these aspects to the  individual.

Finally, the current reconceptualization of the personality disorder nomenclature that is emerging with the DSM-V (American Psychiat- ric Association, 2011) points toward a more dimensional classification of personality dys- function. Utilization of the GAM and assess- ment of relevant knowledge structures is con- sistent with this approach and would assist in the quantification of trait-based aggression, im- proving the measurement and categorization of individuals. Furthermore, in those instances where personality disorder is used as a criterion to determine inclusion or exclusion from mental health treatment or incapacitation (e.g., the United Kingdom’s Dangerous and Severe Per- sonality Disorder program), the present review suggests that an assessment of core GAM spe- cific knowledge-structures represents a more valid and theoretically coherent strategy for identifying people with personality disorder who are at risk for violence rather than rely on diagnosis per se.

 

Conclusion

A large body of literature has accumulated that suggests violence is a clinically relevant correlate of certain personality disorders. How- ever, to date, knowledge and investigation in this area has been impeded by conceptual and methodological barriers, meaning that consider- able uncertainty still exists in terms of how personality disorder operates to influence vio- lence. One way that these difficulties may be resolved is by utilizing current psychological theories of aggression, in particular, the GAM, which was developed to account for differences in aggression propensity across individuals. A review of the available literature in this area indicates that the GAM has relevance to the study of the violent behavior of people with personality disorder. This analysis may also as- sist clinicians focus their assessment and inter- vention efforts for violent individuals with per- sonality disorder.

 

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnos- tic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC:  Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2011). DSM-5 devel- opment: Personality and Personality Disorders. Re- trieved from http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevisions/ Pages/PersonalityandPersonalityDisorders.aspx

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27– 51.

Anderson, C. A., & Carnagey, N. L. (2004). Violent evil and the general aggression model. In A. G. Miller (Ed.), The social psychology of good and evil (pp. 168 –192). New York: Guilford  Press.

Anderson, C. A., Gentile, D. A., & Buckley, K. E. (2007). Violent video game effects on children and adolescents: Theory, research, and public policy. New York: Oxford University  Press.

Ang, R., Ong, E., Lim, J., & Lim, E. (2010). From narcissistic exploitativeness to bullying behavior: The mediating role of approval-of-aggression be- liefs. Social Development, 19,  721–735.

Archer, J., & Haigh, A. (1997). Beliefs about aggres- sion among male and female prisoners. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 405– 415.

Ball, S. A., & Cecero, J. J. (2001). Addicted patients with  personality  disorders:  Traits,  schemas,  and

presenting problems. Journal of Personality Dis- orders, 15, 72– 83.

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Engle- wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice  Hall.

Barros, D., & Serafim, A. (2008). Association be- tween personality disorder and violent behavior pattern. Forensic Science International, 179, 19 – 22.

Beck, A. T., Freeman, A., & Associates (1990). Cog- nitive therapy for personality disorders. New York: Guilford Press.

Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regula- tion of anger and aggression: A cognitive- neoassociationistic analysis. American Psycholo- gist, 45, 494 –503.

Berman, M. E., Fallon, A. E., & Coccaro, E. F. (1998). The relationship between personality psy- chopathology and aggressive behavior in research volunteers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 651– 658.

Bernstein, D. P., Arntz, A., & de Vos, M. (2007). Schema focused therapy in forensic settings: The- oretical model and recommendations for best clin- ical practice. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 6, 169 –183.

Bettencourt, B. A., Talley, A., Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 751–777.

Blackburn, R., & Coid, J. W. (1999). Empirical clus- ters of DSM–III personality disorders in violent offenders. Journal of  Personality Disor- ders, 13, 18.

Blair, R. J. (1995). A cognitive developmental ap- proach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1–29.

Bonta, J., Law, M., & Hanson, K. (1998). The pre- diction of criminal and violent recidivism among mentally disordered offenders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 123–142.

Calvete, E. (2008). Justification of violence and gran- diosity schemas as predictors of antisocial behav- ior in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- chology, 36, 1083–1095.

Calvete, E., & Orue, I. (2010). Cognitive schemas and aggressive behavior in adolescents: The me- diating role of social information processing. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13,  190 –201.

Clark, L. A., Livesley, W. J., & Morey, L. C. (1997). Personality disorder assessment: The challenge of construct validity. Journal of Personality Disor- ders, 11, 205–231.

Coid, J. W. (1998). Axis II disorders and motivation for serious criminal behaviour. In A. E. Skodol (Ed.), Psychopathology and violent crime (pp. 53– 97). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Asso- ciation.

Coid, J. W. (2002). Personality disorders in prisoners and their motivation for dangerous and disruptive behaviour. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 12, 209 –226.

Collie, R. M., Vess, J., & Murdoch, S. (2007). Vio- lence-related cognition: Current research. In T. A. Gannon, T. Ward, A. R. Beech, & D. Fisher (Eds.), Aggressive offenders’ cognition: Theory, research and practice (pp. 179 –198). Chichester, UK: Wi- ley.

Cornell, D. G., Warren, J., Hawk, G., Stafford, E., Oram, G., & Pine, D. (1996). Psychopathy in in- strumental and reactive violent offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 783– 790.

Costa, D. M., & Babcock, J. (2008). Articulated thoughts of intimate partner abusive men during anger arousal: Correlates with personality disorder features. Journal of Family Violence, 23, 395– 402. Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment.   Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74 –101.

Critchfield, K. L., Levy, K. N., Clarkin, J. F., & Kernberg, O. F. (2008). The relational context of aggression in borderline personality disorder: Us- ing adult attachment style to predict forms of hos- tility. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, 67– 82. Daffern, M., & Howells, K. (2009). The function of aggression  in   personality  disordered  patients.

Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 24, 586 – 600. de  Brito,  S.  A.,  &  Hodgins,  S.  (2009). Antisocial

personality disorder. In M. McMurran & R. C. Howard (Eds.), Personality, personality disorder and violence (pp. 133–154). Chichester, UK: Wi- ley.

DiGiuseppe, R., & Tafrate, R. C. (2004). Anger Dis- orders Scale: Manual. Toronto: Multi-Health Sys- tems Inc.

Duggan, C., & Howard, R. C. (2009). The “func- tional link” between personality disorder and violence:  A  critical  appraisal.  In  M.  McMurran  & C. Howard (Eds.), Personality, personality dis- order & violence. Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Friedmann, P. D., Melnick, G., Jiang, L., & Hamil- ton, Z. (2008). Violent and disruptive behavior among drug-involved prisoners: Relationship with psychiatric symptoms. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 26, 389 – 401.

Gilbert, F., & Daffern, M. (2010). Integrating con- temporary aggression theory with violent offender treatment: How thoroughly do interventions target violent behavior? Aggression and Violent Behav- ior, 15, 167–180.

Grann, M., Langstrom, N., Tengstrom, A., & Kull- gren, G. (1999). Psychopathy (PCL-R) predicts violent recidivism among criminal offenders  with

personality disorders in Sweden. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 205–217.

Gray, N. S., MacCulloch, M. J., Smith, J., Morris, M., & Snowden, R. J. (2003). Forensic psychol- ogy: Violence viewed by psychopathic murderers. Nature, 423, 497– 498.

Grisso,  T.,  Davis,  J.,  Vesselinov,  R.,  Appelbaum,

  1. S., & Monahan, J. (2000). Violent thoughts and violent behavior following hospitalization for mental disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clini- cal Psychology, 68, 388 –398.

Guerra, N. G., Huesmann, L. R., & Spindler, A. (2003). Community violence exposure, social cog- nition, and aggression among urban elementary school  children.  Child  Development,  74,  1561–1576.

Hernandez-Avila, C. A., Burleson, J. A., Poling, J., Tennen, H., Rounsaville, B. J., & Kranzler, H. R. (2000). Personality and substance use disorders as predictors of criminality. Comprehensive Psychia- try, 41, 276 –283.

Hiscoke, U. L., Langström, N., Ottosson, H., & Grann, M. (2003). Self-reported personality traits and disorders (DSM–IV) and risk of criminal re- cidivism: A prospective study. Journal of Person- ality Disorders, 17, 293–305.

Hodgins, S., & Cote, G. (1993). The criminality of mentally disordered offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 115–129.

Hodgins, S., Mednick, S. A., Brennan, P. A., Schul- singer, F., & Engberg, M. (1996). Mental disorder and crime: Evidence from a Danish birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 489 – 496.

Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Meehan, J. C., Herron, K., Rehman, U., & Stuart, G. L. (2000). Testing the Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart (1994). Batterer Typology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- chology, 68, 1000 –1019.

Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Stuart, G. L. (1994). Ty- pologies of male batterers: Three subtypes and the differences among them. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 476 – 497.

Howard, R. C. (2009). The neurobiology of affective dyscontrol: Implications for understanding ‘dan- gerous and severe personality disorder’. In M. Mc- Murran & R. C. Howard (Eds.), Personality, per- sonality disorder and violence: An evidence-based approach. Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Howard, R. C., Huband, N., Duggan, C., & Mannion, (2008). Exploring the link between personality disorders and criminality in a community sample. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22, 589 – 603.

Howells, K. (2009). Angry affect, aggression and personality disorder. In M. McMurran & R. C. Howard (Eds.), Personality, personality disorder and violence: An evidence-based approach (pp. 191–212). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Huesmann, L. R. (1988). An information processing model for the development of aggression. Aggres- sive Behavior, 14, 13–24.

Huesmann, L. R. (1998). The role of social informa- tion processing and cognitive schema in the acqui- sition and maintenance of habitual aggressive be- havior. In R. G. Geen & E. I. Donnerstein (Eds.), Human aggression: Theories, research, and impli- cations for social policy (1st ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Johnson, J., Cohen, P., Smailes, E., Kasen, S., Old- ham, J. M., Skodol, A. E., & Brook, J. S. (2000). Adolescent personality disorders associated with violence and criminal behavior during adolescence and early adulthood. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1406 –1412.

Jovev, M., & Jackson, H. (2004). Early maladaptive schemas in personality disordered individuals. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18,  467– 478.

Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., Cima, M., & Chakhssi, F. (2009). Effects of induced anger in patients with antisocial personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 39, 557–568.

Logan, C., & Blackburn, R. (2009). Mental disorder in violent women in secure settings: Potential rel- evance to risk for future violence. International Journal of Law & Psychiatry, 32,  31–38.

McEllistrem, J. E. (2004). Affective and predatory violence: A bimodal classification system of hu- man aggression and violence. Aggression and Vi- olent Behavior, 10, 1–30.

McGlashan, T. H., Grilo, C. M., Sanislow, C. A., Ralevski, E., Morey, L. C., Gunderson, J. G., . . . Brook, J. S. (2005). Two-year prevalence and sta- bility of individual DSM–IV criteria for Schizo- typal, Borderline, Avoidant, and Obsessive- Compulsive Personality Disorders: Toward a hy- brid model of Axis II disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 883– 889.

McNeil, D. E., Eisner, J. P., & Binder, R. L. (2003). The relationship between aggressive attributional style and violence by psychiatric patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 399 – 403.

Mills, J. F., Kroner, D. G., & Hemmati, T. (2004). The measures of criminal attitudes and associates (MCAA): The prediction of general and violent recidivism. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31, 717–733.

Nagtegaal, M. H., Rassin, E., & Muris, P. (2006). Aggressive fantasies, thought control strategies, and their connection to aggressive behaviour. Per- sonality  and  Individual  Differences,  41,   1397–1407.

Nilsson, A. K., Jorgensen, C. R., Straarup, K. N., & Licht, R. W. (2010). Severity of affective temper- ament and maladaptive schema differentiate    borderline  patients,  bipolar  patients,  and   controls.

Comprehensive Psychiatry, 51, 486 – 491. Nordahl, H. M., Holthe, H., & Haugum, J. A. (2005).

Early maladaptive schemas in patients with or without personality disorders: Does schema mod- ification predict symptomatic relief? Clinical Psy- chology & Psychotherapy, 12,  142–149.

O’Brien, M., Mortimer, L., Singleton, N., & Meltzer, (2003). Psychiatric morbidity among women prisoners in England and Wales. International Re- view of Psychiatry, 15, 153–157.

Ogloff, J. R. (2006). Psychopathy/antisocial person- ality disorder conundrum. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40,  519 –528.

Ostrom, T. M. (1994). Foreword. In R. S. Wyer & K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition: Basic principles (pp. vii–xii). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- baum.

Parrott, D. J., Peterson, J. L., & Bakeman, R. (2011). Determinants of aggression toward sexual minor- ities in a community sample. Psychology of Vio- lence, 1, 41–52.

Polaschek, D. L., Calvert, S. W., & Gannon, T. A. (2009). Linking violent thinking: Implicit theory- based research with violent offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 24, 75–96.

Polaschek, D. L., & Gannon, T. A. (2004). The implicit theories of rapists: What convicted offend- ers tell us. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 16, 299 –314.

Posternak, M. A., & Zimmerman, M. (2002). Anger and aggression in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 63, 665– 672.

Rhodewalt, F., & Morf, C. (1998). On self- aggrandizement and anger: A temporal analysis of narcissism and affective reactions to success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 74, 672– 685.

Roberts, A. D., & Coid, J. W. (2009). Personality disorder and offending behaviour: Findings from the national survey of male prisoners in England and Wales. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psy- chology, 21, 221–237.

Ross, J., & Babcock, J. (2009). Proactive and reactive violence among intimate partner violent men diag- nosed with antisocial and borderline personality disorder. Journal of Family Violence, 24, 607– 617.

Seager, J. A. (2005). Violent men: The importance of impulsivity and cognitive schema. Criminal Jus- tice and Behavior, 32, 26 – 49.

Sheldon, K., & Krishnan, G. (2009). The clinical and risk characteristics of patients admitted to a secure hospital-based Dangerous and Severe Personality Disorder unit. The British Journal of Forensic Practice, 11, 19 –27.

Simourd, D. J., & Hoge, R. D. (2000). Criminal psychopathy: A risk-and-need perspective. Crimi- nal Justice and Behavior, 27,  256 –272.

South, S., Turkheimer, E., & Oltmanns, T. (2008). Personality disorder symptoms and marital func- tioning. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- chology, 76, 769 –780.

Specht, M., Chapman, A., & Cellucci, T. (2009). Schemas and borderline personality disorder symptoms in incarcerated women. Journal of Be- havior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 40, 256 –264.

Tadic, A., Wagner, S., Hoch, J., Baskaya, O., von Cube, R., Skaletz, C., Lieb, K., & Dahmen, N. (2009). Gender differences in Axis I and Axis II comorbidity in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology, 42, 257–263.

Tardiff, K., Marzuk, P., Leon, A., & Portera, L. (1997). A prospective study of violence by psychi- atric patients after hospital discharge. Psychiatric Services, 48, 678 – 681.

Thornton, A. J., Graham-Kevan, N., & Archer, J. (2010). Adaptive and maladaptive personality  traits as predictors of violent and nonviolent of- fending behavior in men and women. Aggressive Behavior, 36, 177–186.

Tremblay, P. F., & Dozois, D. J. (2009). Another perspective on trait aggressiveness: Overlap with early maladaptive schemas. Personality and Indi- vidual Differences, 46, 569 –574.

Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2003). “Isn’t it fun to get the respect that we’re going to deserve?” Narcissism, social rejection, and aggression. Per- sonality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 261– 272.

Tyrer, P., Coombs, N., Ibrahimi, F., Mathilakath, A., Bajaj, P., Ranger, M., Rao, B., . . . Din, R. (2007). Critical developments in the assessment of person- ality disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 190(Suppl. 49), s51–s59.

Wallace, C., Mullen, P. E., Burgess, P. M., Palmer, S., Ruschena, D., & Browne, C. (1998). Serious criminal offending and mental disorder. Case link- age study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 477– 484.

Wang, E. W., & Diamond, P. M. (1999). Empirically identifying factors related to violence risk in cor- rections. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 17, 377– 389.

Warren, J. I., Burnette, M., South, S., Chauhan, P., Bale, R., & Friend, R. (2002). Personality disorder and violence among female prison inmates. Jour- nal of American Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 30, 502–509.

Warren, J. I., Hurt, S., Loper, A. B., Bale, R., Friend, R., & Chauhan, P. (2002). Psychiatric symptoms, history of victimization, and violent behavior among incarcerated female felons: An American perspective. International Journal of Law and Psy- chiatry, 25, 129 –149.

Warren, J. I., & South, S. C. (2009). A symptom level examination of the relationship between Cluster B personality disorders and patterns of criminality and violence in women. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32, 10 –17.

Watt, B. D., & Howells, K. (1999). Skills training for aggression control: Evaluation of an anger man- agement programme for violent offenders. Legal  & Criminological Psychology, 4,  285–300.

Weinshenker, N. J., & Siegel, A. (2002). Bimodal classification of aggression: Affective defense and predatory attack. Aggression and Violent Behav- ior, 7, 237–250.

Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (1994). Personality disorders and violence. In J. Monahan & H. J. Steadman (Eds.), Violence and mental disorder (pp. 203–226). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Wong, S. C. (2000). Violence reduction program: Program management manual. Department of Psychology, University of Saskatchewan.

Yang, M., & Coid, J. W. (2007). Gender differences in psychiatric morbidity and violent behaviour among a household population in Great Britain. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiol- ogy, 42, 599 – 605.

Young, J. E. (1994). Cognitive therapy for personal- ity disorders: A schema-focused approach (2nd ed.). Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource  Press.

Young, J. E. (1999). Cognitive therapy for personal- ity disorders: A schema-focused approach (3rd ed.). Sarasota, FL: Professional Resources  Press.

Young, J. E., Klosko, J. S., & Weishaar, M. E. (2003). Schema therapy: A practitioner’s guide. New York: Guilford Press.

Zimmerman. (2005). The prevalence of DSM–IV per- sonality disorders in psychiatric outpatients. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162,  1911–1918.

Comments are closed.