Borderline Personality Disorder: Ontogeny of a Diagnosis

Borderline Personality Disorder: Ontogeny of a Diagnosis

Abstract

Objective: The purpose of this article is to describe the development of the borderline personality disorder diagnosis, highlighting both the obstacles encountered and the associated achievements.

Method: On the basis of a review of the literature, the author provides a chronological account of the borderline construct in psychiatry, summarizing progress in decade-long intervals.

Results: Borderline personality disorder has moved from being a psychoanalytic colloquialism for untreatable neurotics to becoming a valid diagnosis with significant heritability and with specific and effective psychotherapeutic treatments. Nonetheless, patients with this disorder pose a major public health problem while they themselves remain highly stigmatized and largely neglected.

Conclusions: Despite remarkable changes in our knowledge about borderline personality disorder, increased awareness involving much more education and research is still needed. Psychiatric institutions, professional organizations, public policies, and reimbursement agencies need to prioritize this need.

On April 1, 2008, the U.S. House of Representatives unanimously passed House Resolution 1005 supporting the month of May as borderline personality disorder awareness month. The resolution stated that “despite its prevalence, enormous public health costs, and the devastating toll it takes on individuals, families, and communities, [borderline personality disorder] only recently has begun to command the attention it requires.” House Resolution 1005, which was the outcome of public advocacy efforts, drew attention to the disproportion between the high public health significance of borderline personality disorder and the low levels of public awareness, funded research, and treatment resources associated with the disorder. A recurrent theme in this review is the persistence of borderline personality disorder as a suspect category largely neglected by psychiatric institutions, comprising a group of patients few clinicians want to treat.

The review highlights the major clinical, scientific, and public health issues, as well as some of the remarkable personalities, that have shaped the development of this diagnosis. It is necessarily selective. It is organized chronologically, beginning with the period before the diagnosis was used clinically and then dividing the subsequent period somewhat arbitrarily into decade-long intervals. This approach allows the review of the trials and tribulations of borderline personality disorder to proceed within the framework of the changes that were concurrently transforming psychiatry.

Before 1970—From Untreatable Patients to Personality Organization: “A Psychoanalytic Colloquialism”

The identification of patients as “borderline” first arose in an era when the psychoanalytic paradigm dominated psychiatry and our classification system was primitive. At that time classification was tied to analyzability: patients with neuroses were considered analyzable—and therefore treatable—and those with psychoses were considered not analyzable—and therefore untreatable.

The psychiatrists most responsible for introducing the label “borderline” were Stern (1) and Knight (2) . By identifying the tendency of certain patients to regress into “borderline schizophrenia” mental states in unstructured situations, these authors gave initial clinical meaning to the borderline construct. The primary category to which these patients were “borderline” was schizophrenia (37) . Still, until the 1970s the term “borderline” remained a rarely and inconsistently used “colloquialism within the psychoanalytic fraternity” (8) .

The construct took its next major step forward in 1967, when Kernberg (9) , a psychoanalyst concerned with the boundaries of analyzability, defined borderline as a middle level of personality organization bounded on one side by sicker patients who had psychotic personality organization and on the other by those who were healthier and had neurotic personality organization. As such, borderline personality organization was a broad form of psychopathology defined by primitive defenses (splitting, projective identification), identity diffusion, and lapses in reality testing (9) . Kernberg then went on to suggest that these patients could be successfully treated with psychoanalytic psychotherapy (10) .

Beyond the substance of Kernberg’s contributions, their significant impact must be appreciated in part as the product of his authoritative Old-World style and his tireless campaigning on their behalf. He, and to a lesser extent Masterson (11) , who highlighted abandonment issues and poor early parenting, fueled the enthusiastic pursuit of ambitious long-term intensive psychoanalytic psychotherapies for these patients.

Even as this therapeutic optimism was swelling, Klein (12) voiced a cynical counterpoint: “Analysts’ progressive disillusionment with their ability to make permanent change in nonpsychotic patients has been masked by terminological revision. The diagnosis of borderline disorder preserves intact the belief that classical psychoanalysis is the uniformly effective treatment of choice for neurosis, since failures occur only with the borderline patients” (p. 366).

Despite the doubts about this diagnosis’s parentage, important contributions to the borderline construct from these early psychoanalytic observations have endured, among them recognition of these patients’ “stable instability” (13) ; their desperate need to attach to others as transitional objects (14) ; their unstable, often distorted sense of self and others; their reliance on splitting; and their abandonment fears.

1970–1980—From Personality Organization to Syndrome: “An Adjective in Search of a Noun”

In the decade after “borderline” achieved the status of a colloquialism, the advent of descriptive psychiatry and psychopharmacology brought significant changes to psychiatry. The initial effort to describe borderline patients was made by Grinker, an early and powerful advocate of empiricism, and his colleagues in a seminal book entitled The Borderline Syndrome(15) . This development set the stage for the publication of a review of this syndrome’s place within the context of a broader literature in a paper entitled “Defining Borderline Patients” (16) and for the borderline syndrome to become reliably assessable with discriminating criteria (17) . Soon afterward, it entered DSM-III (18) as “borderline personality disorder.”

Borderline personality disorder was official, but what was it? Even before its inclusion in DSM, it had become clear that the disorder was not related to schizophrenia, and the inclusion in DSM-III of another new category, schizotypal personality disorder, finalized this cleavage (19 , 20) . Even without that, the distinctive phenomenology of borderline personality disorder made a spectrum relationship with schizophrenia unlikely. Borderline patients were interpersonally needy, very emotional, and with the exception of occasional lapses in reality testing, they were definitely not psychotic ( Table 1 ). What was also apparent was that they were “difficult” patients and had considerable suicidal risk. Klein (21) described them as “fickle, egocentric, irresponsible, love-intoxicated.” Houck (22) found that they were “intractable, unruly” patients who used hospitals to escape from responsibilities. Thus, these patients attracted pejorative descriptions that discouraged charitable understanding.

 

 

 

 

Given the high levels of comorbid depression in “borderline” patients, some clinicians felt that they had an atypical form of depression (8 , 2931) . Akiskal (32) famously wrote that “borderline was an adjective in search of a noun”—and at that time, in many people’s minds, that noun was clearly “depression.” Others, echoing Klein’s earlier cynicism (12) about the origins of this disorder, felt that borderline personality disorder had been included in DSM-III simply as a conciliatory gesture intended to placate the psychoanalytic plurality, many of whom were opposed to DSM-III’s operationalization of psychiatric diagnoses.

During the 1970s, the literature on treatment for borderline personality disorder was almost exclusively about psychoanalytic psychotherapy. Numerous conferences on psychoanalytic therapy for borderline personality disorder were held, drawing large audiences. The featured speakers all achieved local, regional, or national recognition for what was considered at that time to be their heroic tolerance and remarkable skills (10 , 11 , 3338) . The subsequent flood of books on the disorder ( Figure 1 ) provided compelling accounts of the many serious problems encountered during these therapies, among which were the signal problems of “countertransference hatred” (39) and “negative therapeutic reactions” (10 , 40) . Kernberg (40) wrote that negative therapeutic reactions were common and that they derived from the borderline patient’s “1) unconscious sense of guilt (as in masochistic character structures); 2) the need to destroy what is received from the therapist because of unconscious envy …; and 3) the need to destroy the therapist as a good object because of the patient’s unconscious identification with a primitive and sadistic object” (p. 288). In retrospect, it is notable how the failures of psychoanalytic therapies were explained solely by the borderline patient’s pernicious motivations.

 

Figure 1. Books on Borderline Personality Disorder Published From 1968 to 2008 aa Data from the Library of Congress database, October 2008.

Thus, by 1980, when borderline personality disorder officially entered the DSM classification system, its validity rested primarily and still quite precariously on its clinical utility, and specifically on the ability of the diagnosis to predict a set of clinical dilemmas that were more or less specific to these patients ( Figure 2 ).

 

Figure 2.Treatment Dilemmas Predicted by the Borderline Syndrome

1980–1990—From Syndrome to Personality Disorder: “Wisdom Is Never Calling a Patient Borderline”

During the 1980s, biological psychiatry came to the fore and the recession of psychoanalysis began. After DSM-III defined many disorders with specific and measurable criteria, their validity was now being tested using standards set forth by Robins and Guze (41) . This meant that the validity of borderline personality disorder, like other diagnostic syndromes, was measured via examinations for discriminating descriptors, familiality, longitudinal course, treatment response, and biological markers. The systematic examination of these areas was carried out in numerous clinical research projects on borderline personality disorder. Until 1980, fewer than 15 research reports on borderline personality disorder had been published; in the decade from 1980 to 1990, more than 275 appeared. With only one exception, these projects were conducted without federal funding.

This research showed that the borderline personality disorder syndrome was an internally consistent, coherent syndrome (42 , 43) with a course that differed from those of schizophrenia and major depression (4446) . It also showed that the syndrome was familial and that the prevalence of schizophrenia and depression was not increased in the families of borderline patients (44 , 47 , 48) . The decade’s research also indicated that borderline personality disorder had modest and inconsistent responses to multiple classes of medications (4951) . One conclusion from this considerable body of clinical research was that borderline personality disorder was not simply a variation of—and was probably not closely related to—depression (23 , 52) ( Table 1 ).

The research drew attention to a previously unrecognized diagnostic interface—that with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Here, the differential diagnostic issues were based less on descriptive overlaps than on etiologic considerations. Studies of childhood physical and sexual abuse showed that there were reports of abuse in the histories of 70% of borderline personality disorder patients (53) . This observation occurred at the same time that feminist concerns were raised about DSM-III diagnoses, including borderline personality disorder, that pathologized women or that implicitly blamed victims. Feminist clinicians suggested that descriptions of borderline psychopathology were fueled by men’s anger (54) and that men’s use of this diagnosis for female patients reflected their negative gender biases (55) . Herman wrote that the borderline syndrome was a “disguised presentation hiding underlying PTSD” (53) .

The high frequency of early dropouts from psychoanalytic therapy was now well documented (5658) , as was the infrequency of success with this therapy (5860) . The harm of neutrality, passivity, poor maintenance of boundaries, and countertransference enactment had become clearer, and out of the crucible of popular debates between Kernberg and other analysts (such as Adler [35] and Kohut [ 61 , 62 ]) and relational psychologists (such as Jordan et al. [63] ), the essential role of empathy and support became more widely appreciated.

Thus, the emerging trauma data and the feminist concerns about the borderline label were joined by the ever-growing chronicle of the problems borderline patients allegedly created within psychoanalytic therapies to consolidate a highly pejorative meaning for the borderline diagnosis. From this confluence, Vaillant wrote that “the beginning of wisdom is never calling a patient borderline” (64) .

By this time, sufficient clinical wisdom had accumulated that while we may not have become clear about what we should do, we had learned a lot about what not to do. For example, hospitals knew that borderline patients were not simply feigning symptoms to get admitted; rather, the symptoms were real, but they remitted as a result of the hospital’s “holding” and supportive functions. Indeed, borderline patients’ changing phenomenology could be made coherent by appreciating whether they felt “held” (depressed, cooperative), rejected (angry, self-destructive), or alone (impulsive, brief psychotic experiences) (65) . It was also becoming clearer that nonpsychoanalytic modalities, including group and family therapy and medications, could often be helpful. Thus, the hopes for curative changes that had previously propelled psychoanalytic therapies were quietly being replaced by more pragmatic multimodel approaches that had more modest rehabilitative goals (6571) .

1990–2000—From Unwanted Personality Disorder to Disorder-Specific Treatability: “Would the Patient Be Borderline If She Remitted From a Medication?”

In the 1990s, DSM-IV was published (with only modest changes in the definition of borderline personality disorder) and the biological paradigm had come to dominate psychiatry. During one DSM-IV meeting a new question about borderline personality disorder’s diagnostic integrity was raised: “Would the patient be borderline if she remitted from a medication?” (72) . This was not an easy question to answer. Indeed, borderline personality disorder’s validity was—and still remains—suspect because it has neither a specific pharmacotherapy nor a unifying neurobiological organization from which biological psychiatry can find purchase.

Against this backdrop Siever and Davis (73) proposed two psychobiological dispositions, affective dysregulation (with hyperresponsivity of the noradrenergic system) and behavioral dyscontrol (with reduced serotonergic modulation), which provided a much-needed conceptual and scientific structure for understanding the origins of borderline personality disorder as well as a way to explain borderline personality disorder’s comorbidities and its spectrum relationships with other disorders. On this base, good arguments could be built for viewing borderline personality disorder primarily as an impulse spectrum disorder (74) or an emotional (affective) dysregulation disorder (75 , 76) .

By this time, the cogent considerations about the etiological overlap between borderline personality disorder and PTSD had informed the borderline construct and had usefully shaped its boundaries (24) . Among the clarifying findings were that borderline personality disorder has about 30% comorbidity with PTSD (77) , that borderline personality disorder often develops without a history of significant trauma (78) , that childhood abuse and trauma predispose to many other psychiatric disorders (79 , 80) , and that while exploratory/expressive therapies are even more contraindicated for PTSD than for borderline personality disorder, treating borderline patients as victims of abuse usually made them worse (81) . A subsequent group of studies has now confirmed that while childhood trauma, especially sexual abuse, is related to borderline personality disorder, a history of such trauma is unnecessary and usually does not account for much of the etiological variance (8284) .

The primary differential diagnostic issue had now become bipolar disorder. There was a substantial overlap in the underlying constructs of the two disorders as identified by Siever and Davis’s (73) psychobiological dispositions and in their phenomenology, that is, impulsive/behavioral dyscontrol and affective/emotional instability. Moreover, the bipolar construct was expanding to include spectrum variants—most significantly, bipolar II disorder, for which mania was not required (85) . While the response of borderline personality disorder to mood stabilizers was unimpressive (73) , the disorder’s persisting lack of any distinctive neurobiological base made it an obvious candidate for inclusion in bipolar disorder’s growing spectrum.

Even as these propositions and questions about borderline personality disorder’s biological integrity were gaining attention, the borderline construct was independently receiving creative and groundbreaking advances with respect to the psychosocial aspects of its development and treatment. From England, Peter Fonagy, a Hungarian-born psychologist and psychoanalyst armed with both his skills as a developmental researcher and his contagious energy, enthusiasm, and creativity, introduced studies of early child development and the vicissitudes of caretaking that he postulated set the stage for later development of borderline personality disorder. Beginning with studies of attachment (86 , 87) and building on earlier work by Winnicott (88) and Bowlby (89) , Fonagy postulated that caretakers’ failure to accurately mirror a child’s mental states was responsible for establishing handicaps in knowing one’s self and in empathizing with others—an inability to mentalize that made the child vulnerable to borderline personality disorder.

In this context, the first major stimulus to therapeutics since the psychoanalytic therapy initiative of the 1970s arrived from an unexpected source: dialectical behavior therapy was introduced by Linehan (90) , a self-described “radical behaviorist.” Dialectical behavior therapy was a carefully manualized 1-year outpatient therapy combining well-integrated group and individual therapy components. While targeting the borderline patient’s pattern of self-harm and suicidality, its benefits also extended to less utilization of medication and hospitalization (91) . Among the innovative departures from prior therapies were dialectical behavior therapy’s insistence on split treatment and on identification of treatment goals; its emphasis on validation, skill-building, and here-and-now interventions; its provision of around-the-clock availability; and its definition of the role of the therapist as coach. As important as its empirical support was, and as innovative and learnable as dialectical behavior therapy itself was, one could not have anticipated its impact without appreciating Linehan’s personal role. She was bold, charismatic, and plainspoken. She openly challenged the claims of a psychoanalytic tradition and all other non-empirically based therapies. She inspired a new generation of zealously dedicated—and empirically buttressed—much more effective psychotherapists. Moreover, she introduced a borderline personality disorder-specific therapy that, because it was psychosocial, unwittingly offered an unexpected alternative to the reliance on medication response for satisfying the Robins and Guze (41) validation standard of discriminating treatment response.

Eight years later, a second treatment specifically designed for borderline personality disorder was also shown to be effective. “Mentalization-based treatment” was derived from Fonagy’s developmental research and established its efficacy in an English partial hospitalization program (92) . It was designed to correct the borderline patient’s underlying handicaps in mentalizing by adopting a noninterpretive, “not-knowing,” inquisitive stance intended to facilitate the accurate recognition and acceptance of one’s own and others’ mental states (including the therapist’s).

2000–2009—Borderline Personality Disorder: “A Good-Prognosis Brain Disease”?

The current decade has been associated with a search for the underlying etiological bases for psychiatric disorders. This reflects both a growing impatience with the extensive comorbidities in the current classification system and an excitement about the newly available neurobiological and genetic technologies.

Beginning with the stimulus given by several parent advocacy groups (most notably, the National Education Alliance for Borderline Personality Disorder, and most conspicuously, the indomitable and ubiquitous Valerie Porr) and by the establishment of the Borderline Personality Disorder Research Foundation by a bereaved Swiss family, this decade has seen the adoption of borderline personality disorder by major mental health organizations, such as the National Alliance of Mental Illness, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), and, as noted at the beginning of this article, even the U.S. Congress. In this context, borderline personality disorder seems to have achieved a new legitimacy, at least as a subject for scientific study and for public awareness. Why has this occurred and what does it mean?

Two major findings have greatly affected the borderline construct, one showing that the disorder is significantly heritable and the other that it has an unexpectedly good prognosis. The confluence of these findings is all the more significant because together they seem to defy the expectation that heritable disorders should be among the least changeable. Torgersen and colleagues’ (93) finding of a 68% heritability abruptly invalidated the many theories about borderline personality disorder’s etiology that had focused exclusively on environmental causes. It established borderline personality disorder’s credentials as a “brain disease.”

Signaling the potential yield of the still very limited NIMH-funded research, this decade bore the fruits of two NIMH-funded longitudinal studies, the McLean Study of Adult Development (94) and the Collaborative Longitudinal Study of Personality Disorders (53) . These studies showed that borderline personality disorder has an unexpectedly good course ( Figure 3 ). After completing her seminal long-term follow-up reports (95 , 96) , Zanarini even began to call borderline personality disorder “the good-prognosis diagnosis.” This fact offered enormous encouragement to patients with the disorder even as it raised questions about how those of us in the mental health fields could have been so mistaken. When this finding is combined with the evidence of heritability, it is clear that the DSM criteria are epiphenomena.

 

Figure 3.Longitudinal Course of Borderline Personality Disordera Data from the Collaborative Longitudinal Study of Personality Disorders (unpublished).b Data from the McLean Study of Adult Development (see reference 95).

In 2001, despite the relative absence of an empirical basis, APA prepared guidelines for the treatment of borderline personality disorder (97) . This was done because, as noted earlier, we now knew a lot about what not to do. The guidelines retained a primary role for psychotherapy, but they emphasized the need to enroll patients as collaborators, the need for a primary (i.e., administratively responsible) clinician, and the value of psychoeducation, family involvement, and the use of an algorithm for medications (98) .

Even as the APA guidelines retained a primary role for individual psychotherapy, the role of psychoanalytic psychotherapy and the literature about it had seriously declined (see Figure 1 ). In this decade only four new books on psychoanalytic therapy for borderline personality disorder were published, and three of them were treatment manuals. Most notably these included the manualization of a revised version of Kernberg’s original psychoanalytic therapy, transference-based psychotherapy (99) . In combination with the earlier pioneering development of mentalization-based therapy, the empirical validation of transference-based psychotherapy’s effectiveness (100) revitalized the relevance of psychoanalytic contributions to the treatment of borderline personality disorder.

The Present—Awareness: “Borderline Personality Disorder Is to Psychiatry What Psychiatry Is to Medicine”

House Resolution 1005 states that “it is essential to increase awareness of borderline personality disorder among people suffering from this disorder, their families, mental health professionals, and the general public by promoting education, research, funding, early detection, and effective treatments.” Borderline personality disorder remains terribly and unfairly stigmatized. Most mental health professionals want to avoid—or actively dislike—borderline patients (B.M. Pfohl et al., May 1999 data from unpublished survey). Borderline personality disorder remains far behind other major psychiatric disorders in awareness and research. The difference between its reported prevalences in clinical settings (15%–25%) (28) and in the community (1.4%–5.9%) (101 , 102) indicates that a large number of people with the disorder are undiagnosed and untreated. Research on the disorder receives a total of only about $6 million annually in NIMH funds, less than 2% of the amount allocated to research on schizophrenia (which has a prevalence of 0.4%) (103) and less than 6% of that for bipolar disorder (which has a prevalence of 1.6%) (104) . Despite the significant impact of borderline personality disorder on the course and treatment of anxiety disorders (105) and mood disorders (106) , psychiatric research on these other disorders often fails even to document its co-occurrence.

Research on borderline personality disorder suffers from a lack of young investigators. Most of those who put this diagnosis on the map are approaching retirement, and few of them have the research credentials or funding to nurture a next generation. A growing number of empirically validated treatments for borderline personality disorder exist, but they remain largely unavailable or, when available, are often not reimbursed. More remarkable is that borderline personality disorder still lacks a standing presence in psychiatric training curricula. Appropriate teaching—both academic and clinical—for residents is nonexistent in all but a few institutions. Figure 4 identifies some of the future directions for borderline personality disorder in light of these facts.

 

Figure 4.Future Directions for Borderline Personality Disorder 

The escalating number of books written for nonprofessionals (see Figure 1 ) bears witness to the devastating toll the disorder takes on others. Still unknown are the public health costs of this disorder, but given borderline patients’ heavy utilization of psychiatric services; medical complications; involvement in divorce, libel, and childrearing lawsuits; and violence and sexual indiscretions, the costs can be expected to be tremendous. Also unknown, despite significant advances (107109) , is borderline personality disorder’s core psychopathology and its related neurobiology. While spectrum relationships with bipolar disorder and antisocial personality disorder (110) seem most significant, borderline personality disorder’s defining clinical features remain interpersonal (111) .

With the development of DSM-V under way, borderline personality disorder’s internal coherence and integrity stand on firm ground (112115) , but questions can be expected about whether the label “borderline” should be retained, about whether the diagnosis belongs on axis I instead of axis II, and about whether the diagnosis should be given to adolescents. I believe the term “borderline” has earned honorific status by virtue of its familiarity. As highlighted in this review, it accurately signifies borderline personality disorder’s unclear boundaries while reminding us of an unwanted truth, namely, that psychiatric disorders, like other medical conditions, are heterogeneous and have flexible boundaries. It should not be scapegoated because of this. With respect to the question of whether it should be on axis I or axis II, it belongs on axis I to signify its severity, its morbidity, and its unstable course. But it belongs there too to prioritize its usage and to underscore the need for its treatment to be reimbursed. Use of the borderline diagnosis clearly should be extended to adolescents; its clinical usage in this group is already extensive, its internal coherence and stability are established, and it predicts adult dysfunction as well as adult borderline personality disorder (116) .

In modern psychiatry, borderline personality disorder has become the major container for sustaining the relevance of the mind, an arena that has been endangered by our growing biological knowledge. For example, when a borderline patient cuts him- or herself, the behavior can be understood as providing relief by redirecting intolerable and inchoate psychic pain into physical pain with both a welcome focus of attention and a welcome release of neurohormones. This behavior can just as aptly be considered a breakdown in the ability to mentalize, that is, to keep in mind both the feelings about oneself and another. Such conceptualizations still need to recognize that the intensity or intolerance of the affect has a heritable base—as does, probably, the violent and impulsive cutting behavior itself—and that the patient has a special sensitivity to rejection. But so much is lost if in addition to these explanatory conceptions the cutting is not also seen as an act of self-punishment related to an overly harsh self-judgment reflecting a long-standing perception of “badness.” It is the latter translation of the event that reintroduces the mind. When that sense of badness is further understood as having been derived in part from a family context with much criticism and little nurturance, we begin to give color and distinction to the environmental experiences that account for much of the etiological variance behind the patient’s cuttings. We also begin to recognize a unique personal narrative that allows the person’s life experience to be meaningful and unique and for the person to feel understandable and acceptable—and not as bad as he or she thought.

At this time, borderline personality disorder is the only major psychiatric disorder for which psychosocial interventions remain the primary treatment. Residents who go into psychiatry with an interest in personal involvements, in seeing themselves in their patient’s experiences and their selves as therapeutic tools, now can find few other places in psychiatry to actualize these aspirations. Residents and other mental health professionals who make a serious investment in treating patients with borderline personality disorder can expect to become proud of their professional skills and of their personal growth in tolerance and empathy and to experience a highly personal, deeply appreciated, life-changing role for their patients.

Received Dec. 16, 2008; revision received Feb. 10, 2009; accepted Feb. 17, 2009 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08121825). From McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Gunderson, McLean Hospital, 115 Mill St., Belmont, MA 02478; psychosocial@mcleanpo.mclean.org (e-mail).

Dr. Gunderson reports no competing interests.

Supported by NIMH grants MH400130 and MH400122. The author thanks Mike Stone, M.D., and Larry Siever, M.D., for their helpful comments on this manuscript.

References1. Stern A: Psychoanalytic investigation and therapy in the borderline group of neuroses. Psychoanal Q 1938; 7:467–489Google Scholar

2. Knight R: Borderline states. Bull Menninger Clin 1953; 17:1–12Google Scholar

3. Rorschach H: Psychodiagnostics, 5th ed. Bern, Switzerland, Huber, 1942Google Scholar

4. Hoch P, Polatin P: Pseudoneurotic forms of schizophrenia. Psychiatr Q 1949; 23:248–276Google Scholar

5. Rado S (ed): Psychoanalysis of Behavior, vol 2. New York, Grune & Stratton, 1962Google Scholar

6. Frosch J: The psychotic character: clinical psychiatric considerations. Psychiatr Q 1964; 38:1–16Google Scholar

7. Kety SS, Rosenthal DH, Wender P, Schulsinger F: The types and prevalence of mental illness in the biological and adoptive families of adopted schizophrenics, in The Transmission of Schizophrenia. Edited by Rosenthal D, Kety S. New York, Pergamon Press, 1968, pp 345–362Google Scholar

8. Stone MH: The Borderline Syndromes. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1980Google Scholar

9. Kernberg O: Borderline personality organization. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 1967; 15:641–685Google Scholar

10. Kernberg O: The treatment of patients with borderline personality organization. Int J Psychoanal 1968; 49:600–619Google Scholar

11. Masterson J: Treatment of the Borderline Adolescent: A Developmental Approach. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1972Google Scholar

12. Klein D: Psychopharmacological treatment and delineation of borderline disorders, in Borderline Personality Disorders: The Concept, the Syndrome, the Patient. Edited by Hartocollis P. New York, International Universities Press, 1977, pp 365–384Google Scholar

13. Schmideberg M: The borderline patient, in American Handbook of Psychiatry, vol 1. Edited by Arieti S. New York, Basic Books, 1959, pp 398–416Google Scholar

14. Modell A: Primitive object relations and the predisposition to schizophrenia. Int J Psychoanal 1963; 44:282–292Google Scholar

15. Grinker R, Werble B, Drye R: The Borderline Syndrome: A Behavioral Study of Ego Functions. New York, Basic Books, 1968Google Scholar

16. Gunderson JG, Singer MT: Defining borderline patients: an overview. Am J Psychiatry 1975; 132:1–10Google Scholar

17. Gunderson JG, Kolb JE: Discriminating features of borderline patients. Am J Psychiatry 1978; 135:792–796Google Scholar

18. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd ed. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1980Google Scholar

19. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Gibbon M: Crossing the border into borderline personality and borderline schizophrenia: the development of criteria. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1979; 36:17–24Google Scholar

20. Siever LJ, Gunderson JG: Genetic determinants of borderline conditions. Schizophr Bull 1979; 5:59–86Google Scholar

21. Klein D: Drug therapy as a means of syndrome identification and nosological revision, in Psychopathy and Psychopharmacology. Edited by Cole J, Freeman A, Friedhoff A. Baltimore, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1972Google Scholar

22. Houck JH: The intractable female patient. Am J Psychiatry 1972; 129:27–31Google Scholar

23. Gunderson JG, Phillips KA: A current view of the interface between borderline personality disorder and depression. Am J Psychiatry 1991; 148:967–975Google Scholar

24. Gunderson JG, Sabo AN: The phenomenological and conceptual interface between borderline personality disorder and PTSD. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:19–27Google Scholar

25. Koenigsberg HW, Harvey PD, Mitropoulou V, Schmeidler J, New AS, Goodman M, Silverman JM, Serby M, Schopick F, Siever LJ: Characterizing affective instability in borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:784–788Google Scholar

26. Paris J, Gunderson JG, Weinberg I: The interface between borderline personality disorder and bipolar spectrum disorders. Compr Psychiatry 2007; 48:145–154Google Scholar

27. White CN, Gunderson JG, Zanarini MC, Hudson JI: Family studies of borderline personality disorder: a review. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2003; 11:8–19Google Scholar

28. Gunderson JG, Links P: Borderline Personality Disorder: A Clinical Guide, 2nd ed. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, Inc, 2008Google Scholar

29. Klein D: Psychopharmacology and the borderline patients, in Borderline States in Psychiatry. Edited by Mack I. New York, Grune & Stratton, 1975, pp 75–92Google Scholar

30. Stone M: Contemporary shift of the borderline concept from a sub-schizophrenic disorder to a subaffective disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1979; 2:577–594Google Scholar

31. Akiskal HS: Subaffective disorders: dysthymic, cyclothymic, and bipolar II disorders in the “borderline” realm. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1981; 4:25–46Google Scholar

32. Akiskal HS, Chen SE, Davis GC, Puzantian VR, Kashgarian M, Bolinger JM: Borderline: an adjective in search of a noun. J Clin Psychiatry 1985; 46:41–48Google Scholar

33. Chessick RD: Intensive Psychotherapy of the Borderline Patient. New York, Jason Aronson, 1977Google Scholar

34. Giovacchini PL: The psychoanalytic paradox: the self as a transitional object. Psychoanal Rev 1984; 71:81–104Google Scholar

35. Adler G: Borderline Psychopathology and Its Treatment. New York, Jason Aronson, 1986Google Scholar

36. Searles HF: My Work With Borderline Patients. Northvale, NJ, Jason Aronson, 1986Google Scholar

37. Grotstein JS: Transitional phenomena and the dilemma of the me/not me interface, in The Solace Paradigm: An Eclectic Search for Psychological Immunity. Edited by Horton PC, Gewirtz H, Kreutter KJ. Madison, Conn, International Universities Press, 1988, pp 59–74Google Scholar

38. Akhtar S: Broken Structures: Severe Personality Disorders and Their Treatment. Northvale, NJ, Jason Aronson, 1992Google Scholar

39. Maltsberger JT, Buie DH: Countertransference hate in the treatment of suicidal patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1974; 30:625–633Google Scholar

40. Kernberg OF: Structural change and its impediments, in Borderline Personality Disorders. Edited by Hartocollis P. New York, International Universities Press, 1977, pp 275–306Google Scholar

41. Robins E, Guze SB: Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: its application to schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1970; 126:983–987Google Scholar

42. Hurt SW, Hyler SE, Frances A, Clarkin JF, Brent R: Assessing borderline personality disorder with self-report, clinical interview, or semistructured interview. Am J Psychiatry 1989; 141:1228–1231Google Scholar

43. Clarkin JF, Hull JW, Hurt SW: Factor structure of borderline personality disorder criteria. J Pers Disord 1993; 7:137–143Google Scholar

44. Pope HG Jr, Jonas JM, Hudson JI, Cohen BM, Gunderson JG: The validity of DSM-III borderline personality disorder: a phenomenologic, family history, treatment response, and long-term follow-up study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40:23–30Google Scholar

45. McGlashan TH: The Chestnut Lodge follow-up study, III: long-term outcome of borderline personalities. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:20–30Google Scholar

46. Paris J, Brown R, Nowlis D: Long-term follow-up of borderline patients in a general hospital. Compr Psychiatry 1987; 28:530–535Google Scholar

47. Loranger AW, Oldham JM, Tulis EH: Familial transmission of DSM-III borderline personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982; 39:795–799Google Scholar

48. Links PS, Steiner M, Huxley G: The occurrence of borderline personality disorder in the families of borderline patients. J Pers Disord 1988; 2:14–20Google Scholar

49. Cowdry RW, Gardner DL: Pharmacotherapy of borderline personality disorder: alprazolam, carbamazepine, trifluoperazine, and tranylcypromine. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:111–119Google Scholar

50. Soloff PH, George A, Nathan RS, Schulz PM, Ulrich RF, Perel JM: Progress in pharmacotherapy of borderline disorders: a double-blind study of amitriptyline, haloperidol, and placebo. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:691–697Google Scholar

51. Links PS, Steiner M, Boiago I, Irwin D: Lithium therapy for borderline patients: preliminary findings. J Pers Disord 1990; 4:173–181Google Scholar

52. Gunderson JG, Elliott GR: The interface between borderline personality disorder and affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1985; 142:277–288Google Scholar

53. Herman J: Trauma and Recovery. New York, Basic Books, 1992Google Scholar

54. Stiver IP: The meaning of care: reframing treatment models, in Women’s Growth in Connection: Writings from the Stone Center. Edited by Jordon JV, Kaplan AG, Miller JB, Stiver IP, Surrey JL. New York, Guilford Press, 1991, pp 250–267Google Scholar

55. Henry KA, Cohen CI: The role of labeling processes in diagnosing borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140:1527–1529Google Scholar

56. Skodol AE, Shea MT, McGlashan TH, Gunderson JG, Morey LC, Sanislow CA, Bender DS, Grilo CM, Zanarini MC, Yen S, Pagano ME, Stout RI: The Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study (CLPS): overview and implications. J Pers Disord 2005; 19:487–504Google Scholar

57. Gunderson JG, Frank AF, Ronningstam EF, Wachter S, Lynch VJ, Wolf PJ: Early discontinuance of borderline patients from psychotherapy. J Nerv Ment Dis 1989; 177:38–42Google Scholar

58. Waldinger RJ, Gunderson JG: Completed psychotherapies with borderline patients. Am J Psychotherapy 1984; 38:190–202Google Scholar

59. Waldinger RJ, Gunderson JG: Effective Psychotherapy With Borderline Patients. New York, Macmillan, 1987Google Scholar

60. Wallerstein R: Forty-Two Lives in Treatment. New York, Guilford, 1986Google Scholar

61. Kohut H: The Analysis of the Self. New York, International Universities Press, 1971Google Scholar

62. Kohut H: The Restoration of the Self. New York, International Universities Press, 1977Google Scholar

63. Jordan JV, Kaplan AG, Miller JB, Surrey JL, Stiver IP: Women’s Growth in Connection: Writings From the Stone Center. New York, Guilford, 1991Google Scholar

64. Vaillant GE: The beginning of wisdom is never calling a patient borderline. J Psychother Pract Res 1992; 1:117–134Google Scholar

65. Gunderson JG: Borderline Personality Disorder. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1984Google Scholar

66. Soloff P: Pharmacotherapy of borderline disorders. Compr Psychiatry 1981; 22:535–543Google Scholar

67. Kroll J: The Challenge of the Borderline Patient: Competency in Diagnosis and Treatment. New York, WW Norton, 1988Google Scholar

68. Dawson D, MacMillan HL: Relationship Management and the Borderline Patient. New York, Brunner/Mazel, 1993Google Scholar

69. Silk KR, Eisner W, Allport C, Demars C, Miller C, Justice RW, Lewis M: Focused time-limited inpatient treatment of borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord 1994; 8:268–278Google Scholar

70. Paris J: Borderline Personality Disorder: A Multidimensional Approach. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994Google Scholar

71. Links PS, Mitton JE, Steiner M: Stability of borderline personality disorder. Can J Psychiatry 1993; 38:255–259Google Scholar

72. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR: A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:975–990Google Scholar

73. Siever LJ, Davis KL: A psychobiological perspective on the personality disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1991; 148:1647–1658Google Scholar

74. Links PS, Heslegrave R, van Reekum R: Impulsivity: core aspect of borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord 1999; 12:1–9Google Scholar

75. Livesley WJ, Jackson DN, Schroeder ML: Factorial structure of traits delineating personality disorders in clinical and general population samples. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101:432–440Google Scholar

76. Linehan MM: Dialectical Behavioral Therapy of Borderline Personality Disorder. New York, Guilford, 1993Google Scholar

77. Swartz M, Blazer D, George L, Winfield I: Estimating the prevalence of borderline personality disorder in the community. J Pers Disord 1990; 4:257–272Google Scholar

78. Stone MH: The fate of borderline patients. New York, Guilford, 1990Google Scholar

79. Paris J, Zweig-Frank H, Guzder J: Psychological risk factors for borderline personality disorder in female patients. Compr Psychiatry 1994; 35:301–305Google Scholar

80. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR: Emotional hypochondriasis, hyperbole, and the borderline patient. J Psychother Pract Res 1994; 3:25–36Google Scholar

81. Gunderson JG, Chu JA: Treatment implications of past trauma in borderline personality disorder. Harv Rev Psychiatry 1994; 1:75–81Google Scholar

82. Fossati A, Madeddu F, Maffei C: Borderline personality disorder and childhood sexual abuse: a meta-analytic study. J Pers Disord 1999; 13:268–280Google Scholar

83. Bierer LM, Yehuda R, Schmeidler J, Mitropoulou V, New AS, Silverman JM, Siever LJ: Abuse and neglect in childhood: relationship to personality disorder diagnoses. CNS Spectr 2003; 8:737–754Google Scholar

84. Bandelow B, Krause J, Wedekind D, Broocks A, Hajak G, Ruther E: Early traumatic life events, parental attitudes, family history, and birth risk factors in patients with borderline personality disorder and healthy controls. Psychiatry Res 2005; 134:169–179Google Scholar

85. Akiskal HS, Pinto O: The evolving bipolar spectrum: prototypes I, II, III, and IV. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1999; 22:517–534Google Scholar

86. Fonagy P: Playing with reality: the development of psychic reality and its malfunction in borderline personalities. Int J Psychoanal 1995; 76:39–44Google Scholar

87. Fonagy P, Target M, Gergely G: Attachment and borderline personality disorder: a theory and some evidence. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1999; 23:103–122Google Scholar

88. Winnicott DW: The Maturational Process and the Facilitating Environment. London, Hogarth Press, 1965Google Scholar

89. Bowlby J: Attachment and Loss, vol. 1, Separation: Anxiety and Anger. New York, Basic Books, 1969Google Scholar

90. Linehan MM: Skills Training Manual for Treating Borderline Personality Disorder. New York, Guilford, 1993Google Scholar

91. Linehan MM, Armstrong HE, Suarez A, Allmon D, Heard HL: Cognitive-behavioral treatment of chronically parasuicidal borderline patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:1060–1064Google Scholar

92. Bateman A, Fonagy P: Effectiveness of partial hospitalization in the treatment of borderline personality disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1563–1569Google Scholar

93. Torgersen S, Lygren S, Øien PA, Skre I, Onstad S, Edvardsen J, Tambs K, Kringlen E: A twin study of personality disorders. Compr Psychiatry 2000; 41:416–425Google Scholar

94. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Hennen J, Reich DB, Silk KR: Psychosocial functioning of borderline patients and axis II comparison subjects followed prospectively for six years. J Pers Disord 2005; 19:19–29Google Scholar

95. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Hennen J, Silk KR: The longitudinal course of borderline psychopathology: 6-year prospective follow-up of the phenomenology of borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:274–283Google Scholar

96. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Reich DB, Silk KR, Hudson JI, McSweeney LB: The subsyndromal phenomenology of borderline personality disorder: a 10-year follow-up study. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:929–935Google Scholar

97. American Psychiatric Association: Practice Guideline for the Treatment of Patients With Borderline Personality Disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158(Oct suppl)Google Scholar

98. Soloff PH: Algorithm for pharmacological treatment of personality dimensions: symptom-specific treatments for cognitive-perceptual, affective, and impulsive-behavioral dysregulation. Bull Menninger Clin 1998; 62:195–214Google Scholar

99. Clarkin JF, Yeomans FE, Kernberg OF: Psychotherapy for Borderline Personality: Focusing on Object Relations. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc, 2007Google Scholar

100. Clarkin JF, Levy KN, Lenzenweger MF, Kernberg OF: Evaluating three treatments for borderline personality disorder: a multiwave study. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:922–928Google Scholar

101. Lenzenweger MF, Lane MC, Loranger AW, Kessler RC: DSM-IV personality disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:553–564Google Scholar

102. Grant BF, Chou SP, Goldstein RB, Huang B, Stinson FS, Saha TD, Smith SM, Dawson DA, Pulay AJ, Pickering RP, Ruan WJ: Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV borderline personality disorder: results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Clin Psychiatry 2008; 69:533–545Google Scholar

103. Saha S, Chant D, Welham J, McGrath J: A systematic review of the prevalence of schizophrenia. PLoS Med 2005; 2:413–433Google Scholar

104. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE: Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:593–602Google Scholar

105. Massion AO, Dyck IR, Shea MT, Phillips KA, Warshaw MG, Keller MB: Personality disorders and time to remission in generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, and panic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:434–440Google Scholar

106. Fan AH, Hassell J: Bipolar disorder and comorbid personality psychopathology: a review of the literature. J Clin Psychiatry 2008; 69:1794–1803Google Scholar

107. King-Casas B, Sharp C, Lomax-Bream L, Lohrenz T, Fonagy P, Montague PR: The rupture and repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Science 2008; 321:806–810Google Scholar

108. New AS, Goodman M, Triebwasser J, Siever LJ: Recent advances in the biological study of personality disorders. Psychiatr Clin N Am 2008; 31:441–461Google Scholar

109. Silbersweig D, Clarkin JF, Goldstein M, Kernberg OF, Tuescher O, Levy KN, Brendel G, Pan H, Beutel M, Pavony MT, Epstein J, Lenzenweger MF, Thomas KM, Posner MI, Stern E: Failure of frontolimbic inhibitory function in the context of negative emotion in borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:1832–1841Google Scholar

110. Torgersen S, Czajkowski N, Jacobson K, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Roysamb E, Neale MC, Kendler KS: Dimensional representation of DSM-IV cluster B personality disorders in a population-based sample of Norwegian twins: a multivariate study. Psychol Med 2008; 11:1617–1625Google Scholar

111. Gunderson JG: Disturbed relationships as a phenotype for borderline personality disorder (commentary). Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:1637–1640Google Scholar

112. New AS, Triebwasser J, Charney DS: The case for shifting borderline personality disorder to axis I. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64:653–659Google Scholar

113. Sanislow CA, Grilo CM, Morey LC, Bender DS, Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Shea MT, Stout RL, Zanarini MC, McGlashan TH: Confirmatory factor analysis of DSM-IV criteria for borderline personality disorder: findings from the Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:284–290Google Scholar

114. Johansen M, Karterud S, Pedersen G, Gude T, Falkum E: An investigation of the prototype validity of the borderline DSM-IV construct. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2004; 109:289–298Google Scholar

115. Fossati A, Maffei C, Bagnato M, Donati D, Namia C, Novella L: Latent structure analysis of DSM-IV borderline personality disorder criteria. Compr Psychiatry 1999; 40:72–79Google Scholar

116. Winograd G, Cohen P, Chen H: Adolescent borderline symptoms in the community: prognosis for functioning over 20 years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49:933–941Google Scholar

Comments are closed.