
Narcissism as a Moderator of Satisfaction with Body Image in Young Women with Extreme Underweight and Obesity
Published: May 11, 2015 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126724
Abstract
Objective
Body weight and age constitute main determinants of body image in women. We analyzed the role of narcissism as a moderator of body image in young women representing various extremes of body weight.
Methods
The study included 325 women between 18 and 35 years, qualified into three BMI categories: obese women (BMI > 30.0, n = 72), severely underweight women who did not satisfy the remaining criteria of anorexia (BMI < 17.5, n = 85), and women with normal body weight (21.7 < “ideal BMI” > 22.7, n = 168). Satisfaction with body image was determined with Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire and Body Esteem Scale, while narcissism was measured with Narcissistic Personality Inventory.
Citation: Lipowska M, Lipowski M (2015) Narcissism as a Moderator of Satisfaction with Body Image in Young Women with Extreme Underweight and Obesity. PLoS ONE 10(5): e0126724. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126724
Academic Editor: Cosimo Urgesi, University of Udine, ITALY
Received: June 9, 2013; Accepted: April 7, 2015; Published: May 11, 2015
Copyright: © 2015 Lipowska, Lipowski. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited
Funding: The work was supported from statutory funds of the Institute of Psychology, University of Gdansk and the Gdansk University of Physical Education and Sport.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Perception of one’s body as attractive is associated not only with its objective parameters (height, weight and proportions) [1–5], but also with cultural canons [6–10] and fashion [11–13]. Furthermore, one’s self-satisfaction with body image is also modulated by a subjective attitude to bodily sphere, which in turn is determined by individual characteristics, such as personality profile [14,15], general self-esteem [16], psychological gender [17,18], optimism [19], locus of control [20,21] and narcissism [22–24].
Research suggests [25] that self-attitude to one’s body, which is based on cognitive, behavioral, and emotional components and the subjective determination of the size of various body parts, represents two relatively independent elements of body image. Age and gender are the two factors which most significantly diversify the way the body is felt and perceived, as well as the role of this process. According to the general developmental trend, the level of body-esteem changes throughout the lifetime. After the period of reaching puberty and then adulthood, when it falls quite deeply due to external opinions [26–28], it gradually increases during middle adulthood [29–32], and plummets at the threshold of the old age as a result of confrontation with the evident signs of aging and the general, often negative, opinions about effects of aging [33]. During the period of adolescence and adulthood, beauty is associated mostly with interpersonal success and constitutes an important determinant of social prestige and appreciation; as seems to emanate from each advertisement [34].
Only few psychological processes are more gendered than body image. Men and women differ in both content and degree of body dissatisfaction and body change behaviors [34–37]. Currently gender differences are clear and feasible: while females focus on body weight and body shape, males center around the muscular apparatus [26,38,39]. However the most important difference refers to the subjective significance of physical attractiveness weighted by women and men [37,40,41].
Negative self-attitude of women to their bodies is usually associated with low self-esteem which is observed irrespective of age [30,42,43]. Women are dissatisfied with certain aspects of their image and can even consider their body weight as a determinant of their image [37,44]. Current fashion of slenderness has been reflected by the popularity of various slimming diets; they are most commonly applied to address the appearance of any “defects” of the body and are used by individuals with feelings of being obese, even despite the lack of objective reasons [45–47]. Nevertheless, concluding that body mass is being overestimated and we represent a community of slim individuals, who just assess themselves inadequately, would be a misinterpretation. Unfortunately, there is a growing evidence of obesity being a civilization-related problem of the 21st century. Interestingly, the promotion of slim or even cachectic silhouette in the media is associated with higher incidence of eating disorders; although, it is accompanied by a rapid increase in the number of obese individuals, and the worldwide obesity rate has more than doubled since 1980 [48]. In this matter, we face the so-called “scissors phenomenon” – growing number of underweight women and girls on one hand, and increasing risk of overweight on the other. Aside from individuals suffering from overweight or obesity, who should lose weight and have a history of multiple unsuccessful attempts at accomplishing this goal, there is a growing number of subjects trying to lose weight (frequently with success) despite evident underweight. While only 3% of Poles are severely underweight, as many as 30% of Polish women and 45% of men are overweight (BMI 25.00–29.99) and 15% and 16% of them are obese (BMI > 30) [49]. In United States, 33.0% of women and 35.5% of men are obese [50], while 1.7% are underweight (BMI < 18.5) [51]. Body dissatisfaction is positively correlated with body weight, both in a group of healthy women and in those suffering from eating disorders [52]. The higher is the level of dissatisfaction, the more frequently the body is perceived as larger than it really is; individuals trying to lose weight overestimate their size and this conviction of being fat rather than overweight is the source of dissatisfaction [21,39].
Aside from the parameters being directly associated with one’s bodily sphere, self-satisfaction with one’s body image is also determined by a number of personality predispositions, as well as by the relationship between personality traits and sensitivity to suggestions from others, including mass media [53,54]. By concentrating on their image, many individuals lose touch with reality and apply various beauty treatments or surgical corrections despite receiving feedback from close relations indicating that they worsen rather than improve their appearance [55]. Additionally, not infrequently, despite the lack of objective reasons, such individuals insist on being complemented on their appearance. Such insistence on appreciation and admiration from others despite lacking merits is one of the features of narcissism, understood as a personality dimension [56]. “Narcissism” has unambiguous negative connotation in everyday use and refers to those who have fallen in love with himself/herself and love only himself/herself. However, being proud and satisfied with ourselves and our image is not a sign of any pathology, but rather a form of self-acceptance required for normal development; we are unable to function and develop normally without it [57]. If an individual manifests his/her self-dependence and is convinced of his/her comprehensive and undisputed knowledge to a degree that he/she is unable to experience fear, humility, joy, grief, jealousy, uncertainty, or helplessness – which has significant negative impact on his/her interpersonal relationships – the pathological narcissism can be suspected [58]. Where is the borderline between the norm and the disorder? Although the normal and pathological narcissism are sometimes considered as the extremes of the same continuum (e.g. [59]), a growing body of empirical data and theoretical meta-analyses suggest that they represent two independent entities [60]. Both types of narcissism are underlied by a need for admiration and recognition, but differ in terms of mature regulatory mechanisms level; narcissistic behaviors can have many forms and be either adaptive or maladaptive [61]. In the case of pathological narcissism relations with other people are predominated by the feeling of jealousy, which is associated with a tendency for devaluing others, for example by judging their skills, lack of interest, and appropriating achievements. Moreover, narcissistic individuals are unable to engage in interpersonal relationships, mostly due to the fear of being dependent on other people, or lack of empathy [62,63]. Maintenance of relationships with other people is based on the manifestation of self-superiority and self-dominance. The narcissistic individual needs people to strengthen his/her self-love, and therefore strives to be surrounded by people who will admire him/her uncritically and strengthen his/her conviction of being someone unusual. The Narcissistic Personality Disorder, the diagnostic criteria of which are included in DSM-5 [64], represents evident example of pathological narcissism. Although the association between normal narcissism and positive self-esteem, well-being [65], high level of aspirations, and leadership skills [66] is emphasized, also a tendency of narcissistic individuals to positive self-assessment of their behaviors despite the presence of contradictory facts is highlighted [67].
Objectives
Since body weight is a principal determinant of female body image, one should compare attitudes to the latter in women with different BMIs corresponding to obesity, severe underweight and ideal body weight.
Furthermore, taking into account that preoccupation with self-appearance can be regarded as one of the signs of narcissism, it would be interesting to analyze the relationship between narcissism, considered as a personality trait, and self-satisfaction with one’s appearance. In view of the theoretical background of narcissism construct, it can be assumed that there is a positive relationship between the level of this trait and positive self-evaluation of one’s body image. Furthermore, it is of interest if narcissism in general, or any of its component traits, can affect the self-perception of one’s attractiveness. Finally, we would like to determine if the impact of narcissistic traits on the self-esteem can be modulated by the degree of one’s body weight.
Researchers analyzing the role of personality predispositions as determinants of self-satisfaction with body image, usually adjust the results for body weight of their subjects. The results of the hereby presented study were not only adjusted for the objective parameter (BMI); we went one step further and enrolled participants representing different BMI categories in order to accurately determine the role of narcissism as a determinant of body image among women with various types of silhouette.
Methods
The protocol of this study was approved by the Ethics Board for Research Projects at the Institute of Psychology, University of Gdansk (decision no. 4/2010). Prior to the study written informed consent was obtained from all participating women.
Participants and Procedure
Verification of the relationship between narcissism and body image requires controlling for variables that can significantly modulate the level of life satisfaction. Obviously, these variables include age [43,44] and gender [37]. Therefore, we decided to include only young women between 18 and 35 years (M = 24.1, SD = 5.5) in our study. Defining such limit of age prevented the confounding effect of this factor. On the basis of predefined inclusion criteria (age and gender) we selected the study group among 4 560 individuals taking part in the population-based study entitled Psychological Determinants of Body Image, including randomly selected Three-City (Gdansk-Sopot-Gdynia; Poland) inhabitants.
The next selection criterion was body weight, mentioned previously as a determinant of self-assessed beauty [25,39]. Our study included 325 young women representing various extremes of body weight. Anthropometric measurements were taken by individuals involved in the project, i.e. its authors and auxiliary personnel. Body Mass Index (BMI), i.e. the ratio of body weight to body height calculated on the basis of objective anthropometric measurements, was the qualification criterion to the analyzed groups. Although this measure is very popular, it should be remembered that it does not reflect the type of body structure, namely the muscle mass to fat mass ratio. Therefore, women who practiced sports on a professional basis were excluded from our study as their results could be overestimated (the weight of muscle tissue is higher than that of adipose tissue). None of the participants have been practicing any sport on a competitive basis, either at the time of the study or in the past. The participants were qualified into three BMI categories: obese women (n = 72, BMI > 30.0, M = 33.13, SD = 3.39), severely underweight women (n = 85, BMI < 17.5, M = 16.64, SD = 1.05), and women with BMI between 21.7 and 22.7 (n = 168, M = 21.52, SD = 0.59), i.e. in the exact center of range corresponding to normal body weight (referred to as “ideal BMI” group). We have selected these three groups, representing the extremes and exact center of the BMI continuum, purposefully. The aim was not to confirm the role BMI plays in determining satisfaction with one’s appearance but rather to identify potential differences in the influence of narcissism on body image perception resulting from the degree of obesity/thinness in extreme cases. Importantly, women with BMI < 17.5 did not satisfy other diagnostic criteria of anorexia included in DSM-IV-TR [68]. Participants qualified to this group were additionally referred to clinical consultation which ultimately excluded anorexia on the basis of comprehensive evaluation. As a result, all women with anorexia were excluded from the study.
Measures
Attitude of our participants towards their bodies was determined with two tests: Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire [69] and Body Esteem Scale [70], while the level of their narcissistic traits was examined with Narcissistic Personality Inventory [71], the most widely used measure of narcissism in normative samples [61]. Using two different tests for self-attractiveness enabled us to determine the overall attractiveness score (in MBSRQ) and its particular components (BES).
The Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire
The Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire [69] comprises 69 questions grouped into nine subscales. Only the subscales Appearance Evaluation and Appearance Orientation were used in our study, since the remaining subscales refer to Fitness and Health. Evaluation subscale verifies the self-attitude (positive/negative) to body image, whereas the Orientation subscale determines if the subject shows any activities to improve his/her body image. The final result of each subscale can be scored as very low, low, moderate, high, and very high. Polish adaptation of MBSRQ is based on the translation of the original version. The original survey was translated into Polish by two independent translators: an English philologist and a psychologist. Subsequently, the optimal version of the translation was developed as a result of consensus between these two specialists, and subjected to back-translation into English by a native speaker who was blinded to the original instrument. The consistency of the back translation and the original version was assessed by a bilingual translator. Due to specificity of Polish language, two separate versions of the questionnaire (for women and men) were developed. However, these versions differ solely in terms of gender-specific grammatical structures.
The Body Esteem Scale
The Body Esteem Scale [70] examines the self-perceived body esteem. The scale comprises 35 items grouped into three, gender specific subscales. The subscales for women include Sexual Attractiveness, Weight Concern, and Physical Condition, whereas the body esteem of men is examined with regards to Physical Attractiveness, Upper Body Strength, and Physical Condition. Each BES statements can be scored using a five-item Likert-type scale, where 1 corresponds to have strong negative feelings, 5 to have strong positive feelings, and 3 represents a neutral midpoint. In women, the Sexual Attractiveness subscale refers to the perception of body components whose image cannot be modified by physical exercise (e.g. shape of lips, breasts). The attitude towards these body parts is associated with the emphasis of female sexuality, and their image can be modified solely by cosmetic procedures (e.g. makeup). In contrast, Weight Concern subscale refers to completely different components of the appearance; namely, body parts whose image can be improved by physical exercise or diet. Finally, the third subscale, Physical Condition, pertains to such parameters as stamina, strength, and agility. Polish adaptation of BES followed the same procedure as described above in the case of MBSRQ. Additionally, a reference range of the scale’s values was redefined upon obtaining consent from the author of the original version [72].
Narcissistic Personality Inventory
Narcissistic Personality Inventory [71], in the form of Polish adaptation by Bazińska and Drat-Ruszczak [73] was used to determine the intensity of narcissism considered as a personality variable. The authors of the Polish adaptation developed it on the basis of questionnaires proposed by Emmons [74] and Raskin & Terry [75]. The initial version of the questionnaire developed by Bazińska & Drat-Ruszczak [73] comprised 44 items, all of them included in the instruments of Emmons [74] and Raskin & Terry [75]. The Polish adaptation was validated in three independent groups of subjects (n1 = 510, n2 = 504, n3 = 818). The final version of the instrument comprises four scales: Need for Admiration, Leadership, Vanity, and Self-Sufficiency, identified as most accurate during the adaptation procedure of the original seven-item scale to Polish conditions (Fig 1). Statistical analyses revealed satisfactory internal consistency of the four-item instrument (Cronbach-alpha for Need for Admiration, Leadership, Vanity, and Self-Sufficiency equal to 0.86, 0.86, 0.76, and 0.70, respectively), and factorial structure of the test provides its high stability. Two of the adapted scales, Self-Sufficiency and Vanity, are nearly identical to their originals, and the remaining two, Leadership and Need for Admiration, resemble their original counterparts (i.e. Authority and Exhibitionism) albeit were added the “manipulation- /entitlement-related” positions left after elimination of items with insufficient psychometric values. The manipulation-related statements, originally included in Exploitative scale, were added to Leadership scale. The entitlement-related statements and the statements emphasizing one’s self-superiority (included in Superiority and Entitlement scales of the original instrument) were added to Need for Admiration scale. In the adaptation of Bazińska & Drat-Ruszczak [73], Leadership corresponds to one’s conviction of having influence on the others, and Need for Admiration is associated with the assumption that admiration and appreciation from the others are not necessarily proportional or dependent on one’s achievements.
The Need for Admiration scale reflects the need of being noted, admired, complemented and famous, and includes an element of entitlement. This scale co-exists with the expectation of life satisfaction, indirect aggression (in women), suspicion (in men), self-consciousness in interpersonal relationships, pursuit of career, and the use of self-complication strategy. The Leadership scale determines the belief in the ability to influence others and self-assessed leadership capabilities. It co-exists with stable, high self-assessment, extraversion, indirect and verbal aggression (in women), purposiveness and self-consciousness in interpersonal relationships, and pursuit of career. The Vanity scale analyzes self-preoccupation with body image, self-admiration of physical image, sense of uniqueness and aestheticism. This scale is associated with high self-assessment, expectation of life satisfaction, and, in women, with a lack of guilt and social naivety. The Self-Sufficiency scale examines the self-conviction of competence, success, and independence. It co-exists with pursuit of career and independence from external, social standards of regulation [73].
Results
We begin the presentation of our findings by describing the differences in body image assessment documented by means of ANOVA in women with various BMI. The results of Levene Test confirmed the homogeneity of variance of all studied variables (0.17 < F < 2.73,. 067 < p <. 843). The results obtained with the Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire suggest that obese women score their body image the poorest (low) in Appearance Evaluation Scale (Table 1), while the scores of remaining participants appear to be moderate [62]. More detailed data was obtained by analyzing the results of the Body Esteem Scale, revealing that our women varied in assessing their image using various subscales of this instrument. It is of note that women with dissimilar BMI did not differ with regards to self-perceived sexual attractiveness; this parameter was scored moderately in all studied groups. It was not surprising that participants with disparate BMI scored differently in the second subscale of BES – Weight Concern. Our assumptions were confirmed by the results of post-hoc analysis (Tukey Test for unequal sample sizes): the slimmest women scored the highest and obese women the lowest (p <. 001 for both the comparisons). In turn, the analysis of intergroup differences revealed that women with BMI < 17.5 did not differ from those with moderate BMI with regards to Weight Concern. Women with “ideal” BMI scored the highest with regards to Physical Condition. In contrast, obese women scored significantly lower in terms of Physical Condition, compared to both the slimmest participants (p =. 027) and those with “ideal” BMI (p =. 007). This clearly suggests that the result of the analysis of variance was mostly determined by the “outlaying” of obese women in Weight Concern and Physical Condition subscales. We did not find any significant differences between women with diverse BMI with regards to the second analyzed scale, i.e. Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire – Appearance Orientation. Of note, all our participants scored moderate in this scale.
In the next stage of our analysis we verified which characteristics of body image (BSE) correlate with overall perception of attractiveness or activities towards improvement of the latter (MBSRQ). The results of inter-correlation analysis are summarized in Table 2. We found that the perception of Sexual Attractiveness is the most strongly correlated with Weight Concern. It is of note that this correlation was the strongest in the case of women with BMI < 17.5. The correlation was weaker in women with “ideal BMI” and decreased to r =. 27 in obese women. Moreover, Weight Concern correlated with Physical Condition in all groups; however, in contrast to Sexual Attractiveness, this correlation was inversed. The strongest relationship was documented in obese women and the weakest in severely underweight participants. Finally, Weight Concern correlated with Appearance Evaluation in all groups; this association in extremely slim and obese women was stronger than in the case of “ideal BMI” group. The correlation between Appearance Evaluation and Sexual Attractiveness pertained mostly to underweight women.

Our principal hypothesis pertained to the assumption that narcissistic traits are important determinants of self-assessed attractiveness of examined women with various BMI. Therefore, we verified if the power of these relationships differed depending on BMI group. However, first of all we tested if women with dissimilar BMI differ with regards to the intensity of narcissistic traits. The homogeneity of variance of all analyzed variables was confirmed with the Levene Test (0.34 < F < 1.18,. 310 < p <. 712). The results of the analysis of variance (ANOVA) suggested that women with diverse BMI differed significantly with regards to Need for Admiration and Vanity (Table 3). However, the intergroup analysis (Tukey post-hoc Test) revealed that women with “ideal” BMI differed from obese women only in terms of Need for Admiration (p =. 041) and Vanity (p =. 033).
The values of correlation between narcissistic traits and physical self-assessment are presented in Table 4. The correlations were the strongest in the case of slim women. All analyzed narcissistic traits correlated positively with Sexual Attractiveness, Weight Concern, and Appearance Evaluation. Furthermore, there was a significant relationship between Vanity and Appearance Orientation. The latter positive correlation (i.e. Vanity vs. Appearance Orientation) was documented in all groups of women. The lowest number of statistically significant correlations between the narcissistic traits and self-assessed image pertained to obese women.

Next we used multiple regression analysis to verify which narcissistic traits exert the strongest effect on the self-assessed attractiveness of women with different BMI. The analyses were performed separately for each self-attractiveness scale and each of BMI categories. The predictors which proved significant on all analyses are presented in Fig 2. Vanity proved the strongest predictor for both Appearance Orientation and Appearance Evaluation; importantly, this association was not modulated by the group (BMI level) as confirmed with the use of Indicator Variables in Regression. Moreover, Need for Admiration proved the significant predictor of Appearance Evaluation in obese women, and dummy variable did not reveal difference in relation to slim women and participants with “ideal” BMI.
The predictors (narcissistic traits) did not form a well-fitted regression model with Sexual Attractiveness. Vanity modulated Weight Concern in obese women but not in those with “ideal” BMI (β =. 18, t = 1.82, p =. 071, dummy variable t = 2.12, p =. 035). Also the analysis of predictors of Physical Condition produced interesting findings: Self-Sufficiency proved the significant predictor in the case of obese women but not in the case of slim participants (β = –.02, t = 0.14, p =. 891, dummy variable: t = 2.19, p =. 029) or those with “ideal” BMI (β = –.03, t = 0.36, p =. 720, dummy variable: t = 2.59, p =. 010). In contrast, Leadership was the significant predictor of Physical Condition in slim women but not in obese women (β = –.25, t = 1.47, p =. 148, dummy variable: t = 2.53, p =. 012).
Discussion
First of all, it should be emphasized that although our study confirmed the assumption on the significant influence of body weight on the self-perception of body attractiveness, the group that stood out was comprised of obese women. Body weight constitutes one of the strongest predicators of self-satisfaction with one’s body image, and individuals with the highest weight usually represent the lowest level of self-satisfaction [39,76]. Unfortunately, this is not a simple relationship and one can talk about the self-perpetuating spiral of dissatisfaction, since the lower is the self-satisfaction with one’s image, the more frequently he/she perceives his/her body as greater than in reality. Thus, individuals who try to lose weight usually overestimate their size [77,78]. Many studies revealed that contemporary culture stigmatizes obese individuals, and this rule applies both to adults and children [78,79]. Long-term exposure to social stigmatization increases the probability that overweight subjects will internalize negative information about themselves, and can be reflected by the feeling of mental discomfort with regards to one’s body image [80,81]. The important role of long-term exposure and stigmatization was confirmed in studies dealing with the self-evaluation of pregnant women in the third trimester of pregnancy, whose body image was generally positive and associated with the positive self-esteem [82]. Undoubtedly, the final weeks of pregnancy are evidently associated with high body weight and unshapely silhouette, but these changes are not reflected by any negative social evaluation of pregnant woman’s image. The awareness of relatively quick possibility of losing excessive kilograms and expecting a child represent factors that protect pregnant woman against decreased self-evaluation related to her image.
More comprehensive analysis of the level of satisfaction pertaining to various components of beauty revealed that studied women did not differ significantly with regards to their Sexual Attractiveness. This finding is consistent with the assumption made by the authors of BES test [70]. The components of beauty that highlight female sexuality, e.g. the shape of lips, or breasts, are not directly related to body weight. Moreover, in the case of dissatisfaction with their image, they can be corrected with various techniques of makeup or even by means of plastic surgeries [83].
The results regarding Weight Concern are particularly interesting since documented intergroup differences reflected significantly lower level of satisfaction in obese women. Although this finding is not surprising since obese women usually declare problems with weight control, as proved by frequent unsuccessful attempts to lose weight [21,77], one should emphasize the high level of weight control experienced by underweight women. This finding seems contradictory to the published research on body image among women with anorexia [70,84], who usually declare low levels of satisfaction with their weight control. It should be noted, however, that although our study included women with BMI below 17.5, none of them was diagnosed with anorexia nervosa. The evidence of underweight or even cachexia (the lowest BMI documented in our group was equal to 14.3) is alone insufficient to diagnose anorexia according to DSM-IV-TR criteria [68] as low body weight should be accompanied by such findings as impaired body image, and the fear of gaining weight or being obese. Since individuals with eating disorders perceive their bodies as foreign entities and both the awareness of their body experiences and attitude towards the body are impaired, they are unable to internalize the objective information on low or reduced body weight. Slim women who participated in our study were aware of their body weight and showed high levels of its control. Moreover, these women scored their Physical Condition at a similar level as participants with “ideal BMI”, reiterating the recently frequent finding of a relationship between extremely low body weight and care of physical condition both in anorexia patients and in athletes (e.g. rhythmic gymnastics) without the diagnosis of this disorder [85,86]. The relationships between Physical Condition score and general satisfaction with body image or weight control could be interpreted as a positive finding, i.e. as an increasing awareness of the role played by physical activity and healthy diet in determining and maintaining slender and healthy silhouette. However, this interpretation is contradicted by the lack of significant association between the involvement in improving one’s image and physical condition or weight control. This is particularly surprising in the case of obese women; on the other hand, it can be noted that the objectively confirmed overweight is not necessarily associated with active involvement in silhouette improvement. As many as 76% of our women who exercise in a fitness club have normal BMI, and only 15% of them represent overweight or obesity [87]. This is mostly associated with two absolutely different trends promoted in mass media: creating fashion for slim and fit silhouette on one hand, and satisfying all culinary caprices and sedentary lifestyle on the other. Such contradictory information, especially in the case of children and adolescents growing up in the television era, is reflected by the creation of an image of slim person with hamburger in one hand and a bottle of Coca-Cola (obviously, Cola light) in the other, and ingesting a chocolate bar in the case of sudden hunger, obviously with no negative consequences to his/her silhouette [88].
Paradoxically, it is more difficult for obese individuals to decide to lose weight than for slimmer individuals as losing several or several dozen kilograms by evidently overweight individual requires very intensive involvement. Also, despite reducing weight such person is still perceived as fat. Consequently, why should he/she make an effort if it will result solely in being a “slimmer fat person”? In the case of obese persons reducing the number of ingested calories, being on a rigorous diet, or pursuing unrealistic weight correction are not enough; also, the modification of life style, along with resultant improvement in physical fitness and health, are necessary. Health cannot be equaled to the loss of excessive kilograms; slimmer does not necessarily correspond to healthier [77,80]. Frequently, the decision upon initiating weight loss does not result from the factual physiological or medical need; dieting becomes a part of life style where a woman should refuse eating since such behavior affirms her high level of internal control and care of her body.
Feedback received from others, and above all the need for positive assessment, constitute the component of narcissism analyzed as a personality trait. Previous studies revealed a positive correlation between self-assessment of one’s body and narcissism [22,23], and therefore we focused on investigating the relationship between the level of various narcissistic traits and the level of self-satisfaction with body image in women with various BMI. Observed in our study lack of significant differences between underweight women and women with normal weight with regards to theirs narcissism intensity was partially confirmed by other authors [89]. Obese women are characterized by rather low level of self-esteem, and therefore this group does not show high levels of narcissism considered as a personality trait. Obesity is a chronic condition; most obese young women were previously obese girls which exerts significant effect on the development of their personality. More frequently, eating disorders characterized by extremely low body weight, are rather associated with narcissistic defense [24]. However, as previously emphasized, despite extremely low weight, the study group of women with BMI < 17.5 did not satisfy any other diagnostic criterion of anorexia. Therefore, they were not characterized by low self-assessment of body, and anxiety- or escape-like attitude to losing weight [85]. Therefore, this group of our participants comprised slim individuals, who were aware of this fact, and moreover, intentionally chose this appearance.
It is not surprising that Vanity was the narcissistic personality trait, which modulated self-perception the strongest. The assumption on this relationship is in fact underlying the theoretical construct of narcissism; it is of note, however, that depending on BMI level, this trait influenced various components of physical attractiveness in our women. Irrespective of body weight, Vanity modulated the overall self-satisfaction with one’s appearance; however, only in obese women higher level of Vanity seemed to attenuate the Weight Concern score. Such association was also revealed in a study on the effectiveness of losing weight after bariatric surgery, in which the level of narcissism correlated inversely with the number of lost kilograms [90]. In contrast, in the case of women with severe underweight, we observed significant association between this trait and involvement in activities aimed at image improvement. It is frequently postulated that slim women, both with no features of anorexia and with diagnosis of this disorder, are heavily involved in sport activity [85,87,91]. It is even postulated that it is involvement in physical activity which is stronger associated with positive body image and positive health perceptions rather than objective health status or involvement in other health practices [92]. Interestingly, in the case of obese women, the enhanced activities aimed at body image improvement result mostly from pursuing social acceptance. The need for being liked by others rather than a simple vanity stimulates them to such activities. We postulate that this is not only associated with the need for improving the attractiveness of body image, but also with the fact that pursuing slimness is socially perceived as a sign of self-control and is even required by others [80]. Such interpretation was confirmed by the association between Self-Sufficiency and Physical Condition, pointing to the awareness of the relationship between self-competence or self-success and self-assessment of one’s body [39,77].
The relationship between Leadership and Physical Condition in underweight women should be considered interesting. Although our participants did not satisfy the diagnostic criteria of anorexia, they ascribed significant role to the need for self-control and control over others. This finding constitutes another contribution to ongoing discussion on expanding the definition of eating disorders [85].
Our study provided further evidence confirming that both the objective anthropometric parameters and the level of narcissism are associated with an attitude of young women of their bodily sphere. Having in mind, that one’s attitude to his/her body is defined as early as in the childhood [79,93,94], mostly under the influence of other people [27], the guardians should do their best to prevent a decrease in self-esteem of their children, resulting either from underweight [42] or from excessive attention paid to body image [95].
However, the preventive measures should not be oriented solely at improvement of health, prevention of obesity or underweight, but also center around attenuation of the role of body image as a determinant of narcissism dimensions, such as Need for Admiration and Vanity. High level of narcissism, which was shown to be inversely correlated with subjective well-being [96], combined with lack of self-acceptance to body image, may constitute an important risk factor predisposing to distortion of the latter or promoting development of eating disorders [39,46,52].
Conclusions
Body weight constitutes the determinant of satisfaction with body image in young women. Also narcissism, considered as a personality trait, proved to constitute a significant modulator. However, the role of narcissism as a modulator of self-satisfaction with one’s body varies depending on BMI level: extremely underweight women and obese individuals constitute groups in which narcissism has the strongest impact on the self-satisfaction with body.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: M. Lipowska M. Lipowski. Performed the experiments: M. Lipowska M. Lipowski. Analyzed the data: M. Lipowska M. Lipowski. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: M. Lipowska M. Lipowski. Wrote the paper: M. Lipowska M. Lipowski.
References
- 1. Cash TF, Morrow JA, Hrabosky JI, Perry AA. How has body image changed? A cross-sectional investigation of college women and men from 1983 to 2001. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72: 1081–1089. pmid:15612854
- 2. Coy AE, Green JD, Price ME. Why is low waist-to-chest ratio attractive in males? The mediating roles of perceived dominance, fitness, and protection ability. Body Image. 2014;11: 282–289. pmid:24958664
- 3. Noser A, Zeigler-Hill V. Investing in the ideal: does objectified body consciousness mediate the association between appearance contingent self-worth and appearance self-esteem in women? Body Image. 2014;11: 119–125. pmid:24374074
- 4. Sorokowski P. Did Venus have long legs? Beauty standards from various historical periods reflected in works of art. Perception. 2010;39: 1427–1430. pmid:21180366
- 5. Robles DS. Thinness and beauty: when food becomes the enemy. International Journal of Research & Review. 2009;2: 16–30.
- 6. Bakhshi S. Women’s body image and the role of culture: A review of the literature. Europe’s J Psychol. 2011;7: 374–394.
- 7. Cash TF, Smolak L. Understanding body images: Historical and contemporary perspectives. In: Cash TF, Smolak L, editors. Body Image: A Handbook of Science, Practice, and Prevention. New York: Guilford Press; 2011. pp. 3–11.
- 8. Jackson J, Lyons TC. The perfect body: Men and women negotiate spaces of resistance against beauty and gender ideologies. Women’s Studies J. 2012;26: 25–33.
- 9. Mills JS, Jadd R, Key BL. Wanting a body that’s better than average: the effect of manipulated body norms on ideal body size perception. Body Image. 2012;9: 365–372. pmid:22494959
- 10. Paulk A, Dowd DA, Zayac R, Eklund A, Kildare C. The Relationship Between Culture, Geographic Region, and Gender on Body Image: A Comparison of College Students in the Southeast and Pacific Northwest Regions of the United States. Sociol Spectrum. 2014;34: 442–452.
- 11. Fardouly J, Diedrichs PC, Vartanian LR, Halliwell E. Social comparisons on social media: The impact of Facebook on young women’s body image concerns and mood. Body Image. 2015;13: 38–45. pmid:25615425
- 12. Rudd NA, Harmon J, Heiss V, Buckworth J. Obesity bias and body image: How do fashion and retail students compare to other personal service majors? Int J Fashion Design Technol Edu. 2015;8: 1–9.
- 13. Slater A, Tiggemann M. Media matters for boys too! The role of specific magazine types and television programs in the drive for thinness and muscularity in adolescent boys. Eat Behav. 2014;15: 679–682. pmid:25462026
- 14. Skorek M, Song AV, Dunham Y. Self-esteem as a mediator between personality traits and body esteem: path analyses across gender and race/ethnicity. 2014. PLoS One 9: e112086. pmid:25375238
- 15. Swami V, Tran US, Brooks LH, Kanaan L, Luesse E-M, Nader I, et al. Body image and personality: Associations between the Big Five Personality Factors, actual-ideal weight discrepancy, and body appreciation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2013;54: 146–151. pmid:23198845
- 16. Zeigler-Hill V, Noser A. Will I Ever Think I’m Thin Enough? A Moderated Mediation Study of Women’s Contingent Self-Esteem, Body Image Discrepancies, and Disordered Eating. Psychol Women Quart. 2015;39: 109–118.
- 17. Cella S, Iannaccone M, Cotrufo P. Influence of gender role orientation (masculinity versus femininity) on body satisfaction and eating attitudes in homosexuals, heterosexuals and transsexuals. Eat Weight Disord. 2013;18: 115–124. pmid:23760839
- 18. Murray T, Lewis V. Gender-role conflict and men’s body satisfaction: The moderating role of age. Psychol Men Masculinity. 2014;15: 40–48.
- 19. Dalley SE, Vidal J. Optimism and positive body image in women: The mediating role of the feared fat self. Personality and Individual Differences. 2013;55: 465–468.
- 20. Pokrajac-Bulian A, Živčić-Bećirević I. Locus of control and self-esteem as correlates of body dissatisfaction in Croatian university students. European Eating Disorders Review. 2005;13: 54–60.
- 21. Parzuchowska M, Lipowska M. Locus of control and body image in young women and the effectiveness of dieting. Psychologia Rozwojowa. 2006;11: 113–121.
- 22. Davis C, Claridge G, Cerullo D. Reflections on narcissism: conflicts about body-image perceptions in women. Personality and Individual Differences. 1997;22: 309–316.
- 23. Jackson LA, Ervin KS, Hodge CN. Narcissism and body image. Journal of Research in Personality. 1992;26: 357–370.
- 24. Waller G, Sines J, Meyer C, Mountford V. Body checking in the eating disorders: association with narcissistic characteristics. Eat Behav. 2008;9: 163–169. pmid:18329594
- 25. Rucker CE, Cash TF. Body images, body-size perception, and eating behaviors among African-American and White College Women. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1992;12: 291–299.
- 26. Bucchianeri MM, Arikian AJ, Hannan PJ, Eisenberg ME, Neumark-Sztainer D. Body dissatisfaction from adolescence to young adulthood: findings from a 10-year longitudinal study. Body Image. 2013;10: 1–7. pmid:23084464
- 27. Mitchell SH, Petrie TA, Greenleaf CA, Martin SB. Moderators of the internalization-body dissatisfaction relationship in middle school girls. Body Image. 2012;9: 431–440. pmid:22858554
- 28. Standley R, Sullivan V, Wardle J. Self-perceived weight in adolescents: over-estimation or under-estimation? Body Image. 2009;6: 56–59. pmid:18996777
- 29. Kochan-Wójcik M, Piskorz J. Changes in body perception and evaluation among women from adolescence to adulthood. Psychologia Rozw. 2010;15: 21–32.
- 30. Tiggemann M, McCourt A. Body appreciation in adult women: relationships with age and body satisfaction. Body Image. 2013;10: 624–627. pmid:23954196
- 31. McLaren L, Hardy R, Kuh D. Women’s body satisfaction at midlife and lifetime body size: a prospective study. Health Psychol. 2003;22: 370–377. pmid:12940393
- 32. Ziebland S, Robertson J, Jay J, Neil A. Body image and weight change in middle age: a qualitative study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2002;26: 1083–1091. pmid:12119574
- 33. Ferraro FR, Muehlenkamp JJ, Paintner A, Wasson K, Hager T, Hoverson F. Aging, body image, and body shape. J Gen Psychol. 2008;135: 379–392. pmid:18959228
- 34. Marium J, Iftikhar A. Adolescent girls are hurt more by the body mass than thin-ideal media images of females. J Behav Sci. 2014;24: 29–43.
- 35. Franzoi SL. The body-as-object versus body-as-process: Gender differences and gender considerations. Sex Roles. 1995;56: 13–21.
- 36. Pruis TA, Janowsky JS. Assessment of body image in younger and older women. J Gen Psychol. 2010;137: 225–238. pmid:20718224
- 37. Franzoi SL, Vasquez K, Sparapani E, Frost K, Martin J, Aebly M. Exploring body comparison tendencies: women are self-critical whereas men are self-hopeful. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2012;36: 99–109.
- 38. Tod D, Edwards C. Predicting drive for muscularity behavioural engagement from body image attitudes and emotions. Body Image. 2013;10: 135–138. pmid:23021637
- 39. Gumble A, Carels R. The harmful and beneficial impacts of weight bias on well-being: the moderating influence of weight status. Body Image. 2012;9: 101–107. pmid:21871850
- 40. Gillen MM. Associations between positive body image and indicators of men’s and women’s mental and physical health. Body Image. 2015;13: 67–74. pmid:25682474
- 41. Oehlhof ME, Musher-Eizenman DR, Neufeld JM, Hauser JC. Self-objectification and ideal body shape for men and women. Body Image. 2009;6: 308–310. pmid:19570734
- 42. Radziwiłłxowicz W, Macias M. Self-esteem and achievement motivation level in overweight and obese adolescents. Health Psychol Rep. 2014;2: 132–143.
- 43. Tiggemann M. Body image across the adult life span: stability and change. Body Image. 2004;1: 29–41. pmid:18089139
- 44. Lipowska M, Lipowski M. The evaluation of own attractiveness by females of different age. In: Chybicka A, Kaźmierczak M, editors. The woman in the culture—the culture in the woman. Cracow: Oficyna Wydawnicza Impuls; 2006. pp. 385–402.
- 45. Mącik D, Kowalska-Dąbrowska M. The risk of muscle dysmorphia and the perception of change in retrospective, current and ideal self-image – preliminary study. Health Psychol Rep. 2015;3: 24–34.
- 46. Fayet F, Petocz P, Samman S. Prevalence and correlates of dieting in college women: a cross sectional study. Int J Womens Health. 2012;4: 405–411. pmid:22956885
- 47. Kapka-Skrzypczak L, Bergier B, Diatczyk J, Niedźwiecka J, Biliński P, Wojtyła A. Dietary habits and body image perception among Polish adolescents and young adults—a population based study. Ann Agric Environ Med. 2012;19: 299–308. pmid:22742806
- 48. World Health Organization. WHO Global Database on Body Mass Index (BMI); 2012.
- 49. Central Statistical Office. Statistical Yearbook of the Republic of Poland. Warsaw: Statistical Publishing Establishment; 2011.
- 50. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of obesity in the United States, 2009–2010. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics; 2012.
- 51. Fryar CD, Ogden CL. Prevalence of underweight among adults aged 20 and over: United States, 1960–1962 through 2007–2010. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics; 2012.
- 52. Izydorczyk B. Psychological profile of bodily self features of young Polish women—comparative analysis of bodily self structure of woman with eating disorders and psychosis. Psychiatr Pol. 2011; 45: 653–670. pmid:22220483
- 53. Roberts A, Good E. Media images and female body dissatisfaction: the moderating effects of the Five-Factor traits. Eat Behav. 2010;11: 211–216. pmid:20850054
- 54. Anschutz D, Van Strien T, Engels R. Exposure to slim images in mass media: television commercials as reminders of restriction in restrained eaters. Health Psychol. 2008;27: 401–408. pmid:18642997
- 55. Slevec J, Tiggemann M. Attitudes toward cosmetic surgery in middle-aged women: body image, aging anxiety, and the media. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2010;34: 65–74.
- 56. Brown RP, Budzek K, Tamborski M. On the meaning and measure of narcissism. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2009;35: 951–964. pmid:19487486
- 57. Thomaes S, Reijntjes A, Orobio de Castro B, Bushman BJ, Poorthuis A, Telch MJ. I like me if you like me: on the interpersonal modulation and regulation of preadolescents’ state self-esteem. Child Dev. 2010;81: 811–825. pmid:20573106
- 58. Morey LC, Stagner BH. Narcissistic pathology as core personality dysfunction: comparing the DSM-IV and the DSM-5 proposal for narcissistic personality disorder. J Clin Psychol. 2012;68: 908–921. pmid:22730037
- 59. Watson P, Trumpeter N, O’Leary B, Morris R, Culhane S. Narcissism and self-esteem in the presence of imagined others: supportive versus destructive object representations and the continuum hypothesis. Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 2005;25: 253–268.
- 60. Pincus AL, Lukowitsky MR. Pathological narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6: 421–446. pmid:20001728
- 61. Roche MJ, Pincus AL, Lukowitsky MR, Menard KS, Conroy DE. An integrative approach to the assessment of narcissism. J Pers Assess. 2013;95: 237–248. pmid:23451709
- 62. Nicholls E, Stukas AA. Narcissism and the self-evaluation maintenance model: effects of social comparison threats on relationship closeness. J Soc Psychol. 2011;151: 201–212. pmid:21476462
- 63. Pastwa-Wojciechowska B, Brycz H. The structure of the narcissism and the psychopathy. In: Maliszewski T, Wojtowicz WJ, Żerko J, editors. Anthology of social and behavioral science. Linkoping, Gdansk: University of Gdansk; 2005. pp. 458–471.
- 64. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fifth Edition. Washington: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
- 65. Zuckerman M, O’Loughun R. Narcissism and well-being: a longitudinal perspective. Eur J Social Psychol. 2009;39: 957–972.
- 66. Nevicka B, De Hoogh A, Van Vianen A, Beersma B, McIlwain D. All I need is a stage to shine: narcissists’ leader emergence and performance. Leadership Quart. 2011;22: 910–925.
- 67. Morf C, Rhodewalt F. Expanding the dynamic self-regulatory processing model of narcissism: research directions for the future. Psychol Inquiry. 2001;12: 243–251.
- 68. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition (Text Revision). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.
- 69. Cash TF. Users’ manual for the Multidimensional Body Self Relations Questionnaire (3rd revision); 2000. Available: http://www.body-images.com.
- 70. Franzoi SL, Shields SA. The Body Esteem Scale: multidimensional structure and sex differences in a college population. J Pers Assess. 1984;48: 173–178. pmid:6726603
- 71. Raskin R, Hall CS. The Narcissistic Personality Inventory: alternative form reliability and further evidence of construct validity. J Pers Assess. 1981;45: 159–162. pmid:16370732
- 72. Lipowska M, Lipowski M. Polish normalization of the Body Esteem Scale (BES). Health Psychol Rep. 2013;1: 72–81.
- 73. Bazińska R, Drat-Ruszczak K. Structure of narcissism in the Polish adaptation of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Czasopismo Psychologiczne. 2000;6: 171–188.
- 74. Emmons RA. Narcissism: theory and measurement. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;52: 11–17. pmid:3820065
- 75. Raskin R, Terry H. A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54: 890–902. pmid:3379585
- 76. Loth KA, Mond J, Wall M, Neumark-Sztainer D. Weight status and emotional well-being: longitudinal findings from Project EAT. J Pediatr Psychol. 2011;36: 216–225. pmid:20378686
- 77. Brytek-Matera A. Body image among obese women: consequences and degree of body dissatisfaction, relationship with low self-esteem and coping strategies. Psychiatr Pol. 2010;44: 267–275. pmid:20677445
- 78. Lemon SC, Rosal MC, Zapka J, Borg A, Andersen V. Contributions of weight perceptions to weight loss attempts: differences by body mass index and gender. Body Image. 2009;6: 90–96. pmid:19188102
- 79. Burgess JN, Broome ME. Perceptions of weight and body image among preschool children: A pilot study. Pediatr Nurs. 2012;38: 147–152, 176. pmid:22908457
- 80. Annis NM, Cash TF, Hrabosky JI. Body image and psychosocial differences among stable average weight, currently overweight, and formerly overweight women: the role of stigmatizing experiences. Body Image. 2004;1: 155–167. pmid:18089148
- 81. Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. Obesity and body image. Body Image. 2004;1: 43–56. pmid:18089140
- 82. Kaźmierczak M, Goodwin R. Pregnancy and body image in Poland: gender roles and self-esteem during the third trimester. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. 2011;29: 1–9.
- 83. Sezgin B, Findikcioglu K, Kaya B, Sibar S, Yavuzer R. Mirror on the wall: a study of women’s perception of facial features as they age. Aesthet Surg J. 2012;32: 421–425. pmid:22436235
- 84. Hrabosky JI, Cash TF, Veale D, Neziroglu F, Soll EA, Garner DM, et al. Multidimensional body image comparisons among patients with eating disorders, body dysmorphic disorder, and clinical controls: a multisite study. Body Image. 2009;6: 155–163. pmid:19410528
- 85. Dalle Grave R, Calugi S, Marchesini G. Underweight eating disorder without over-evaluation of shape and weight: Atypical anorexia nervosa? Int J Eat Disord. 2008;41: 705–712. pmid:18523958
- 86. Karr TM, Zunker C, Thompson RA, Sherman RT, Erickson A, Cao L, et al. Moderators of the association between exercise identity and obligatory exercise among participants of an athletic event. Body Image. 2013;10: 70–77. pmid:23092850
- 87. Lipowski M, Buliński L, Krawczyński M. Physical activities among other types of health-related behaviour in people losing weight. Med Sci Monit. 2009;15: CR423–CR428. pmid:19644420
- 88. Krahé B, Krause C. Presenting thin media models affects women’s choice of diet or normal snacks. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2010;34: 349–355.
- 89. Waller G, Sines J, Meyer C, Foster E, Skelton A. Narcissism and narcissistic defences in the eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2007;40: 143–148. pmid:17080446
- 90. Pontiroli AE, Fossati A, Vedani P, Fiorilli M, Folli F, Paganelli M, et al. Post-surgery adherence to scheduled visits and compliance, more than personality disorders, predict outcome of bariatric restrictive surgery in morbidly obese patients. Obes Surg. 2007;17: 1492–1497. pmid:18219777
- 91. Slay HA, Hayaki J, Napolitano MA, Brownell KD. Motivations for running and eating attitudes in obligatory versus nonobligatory runners. Int J Eat Disord. 1998;23: 267–275. pmid:9547661
- 92. Korn L, Gonen E, Shaked Y, Golan M. Health perceptions, self and body image, physical activity and nutrition among undergraduate students in Israel. PLoS One. 2013;8: e58543. pmid:23516503
- 93. Brownell CA, Zerwas S, Ramani GB. “So big”: the development of body self-awareness in toddlers. Child Dev. 2007;78: 1426–1440. pmid:17883440
- 94. Camoes-Costa V, Erjavec M, Horne PJ. Comprehension and production of body part labels in 2- to 3-year-old children. Br J Dev Psychol. 2011;29: 552–571. pmid:21848746
- 95. Hayes S, Tantleff-Dunn S. Am I too fat to be a princess? Examining the effects of popular children’s media on young girls’ body image. Br J Dev Psychol. 2010;28: 413–426. pmid:20481395
- 96. Żemojtel-Piotrowska M, Clinton A, Piotrowski J. Agentic and communal narcissism and subjective well-being: are narcissistic individuals unhappy? A research report. Current Issues Personality Psychol. 2014;2: 10–16.
Discover more from Psychology
Subscribe to get the latest posts sent to your email.